Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-pftt2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-02T19:40:36.333Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Developmental trajectories of anger and sadness dysregulation in childhood differentially predict later borderline symptoms

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 June 2023

Alecia C. Vogel*
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Ben Geselowitz
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Rebecca Tillman
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Deanna M. Barch
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences, Washington University in St. Louis. St. Louis, MO, USA Department of Radiology, Washington University in St. Louis. St. Louis, MO, USA
Joan L. Luby
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
Diana J. Whalen
Affiliation:
Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St. Louis, MO, USA
*
Corresponding author: Alecia C. Vogel; Email: vogela@wustl.edu
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Difficulties with emotion regulation are integral to borderline personality disorder (BPD) and its hypothesized developmental pathway. Here, we prospectively assess trajectories of emotion processing across childhood, how BPD symptoms impact these trajectories, and whether developmental changes are transdiagnostic or specific to BPD, as major depressive (MDD) and conduct disorders (CD) are also characterized by emotion regulation difficulties. This study included 187 children enriched for those with early symptoms of depression and disruptive behaviors from a longitudinal study. We created multilevel models of multiple components of emotional processing from mean ages 9.05 to 18.55 years, and assessed the effect of late adolescent BPD, MDD, and CD symptoms on these trajectories. Linear trajectories of coping with sadness and anger, and quadratic trajectories of dysregulated expressions of sadness and anger were transdiagnostic, but also exhibited independent relationships with BPD symptoms. Only inhibition of sadness was related to BPD symptoms. The quadratic trajectories of poor emotional awareness and emotional reluctance were also independently related to BPD. Findings support examining separable components of emotion processing across development as potential precursors to BPD, underscoring the importance of understanding these trajectories as not only a marker of potential risk but also potential targets for prevention and intervention.

Type
Regular Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press

Introduction

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) is a relatively common and highly impairing diagnosis characterized by affective lability and impulsivity, as well as unstable interpersonal relationships and self-image (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). BPD affects 1–2% of the general population (Torgersen et al., Reference Torgersen, Kringlen and Cramer2001), but prevalence rises to 20% in patients receiving outpatient psychiatric care (Korzekwa et al., Reference Korzekwa, Dell, Links, Thabane and Webb2008) and to 50% in patients receiving inpatient care (Grilo et al., Reference Grilo, McGlashan, Quinlan, Walker, Greenfeld and Edell1998). BPD is associated with severe functional impairment across several domains, including a rate of suicide almost 50 times higher than the general population (Pompili et al., Reference Pompili, Girardi, Ruberto and Tatarelli2005), educational and occupational morbidity, and extensive health services utilization (van Asselt et al., Reference van Asselt, Dirksen, Arntz and Severens2007). Yet, research on developmental precursors leading to BPD lags far behind other disorders such as major depressive disorder (MDD) and externalizing pathology. Additional work is greatly needed that addresses both shared and unique developmental precursors to BPD that help to differentiate developmental trajectories of BPD from other common and co-occurring disorders (Stepp, Lazarus, et al., Reference Stepp, Lazarus and Byrd2016).

Historically, BPD has been viewed as a disorder of adulthood. However, recent work challenges this assumption and recognizes the developmental nature of BPD and associated precursors beginning during childhood. BPD is theorized to develop from interactions between biological vulnerabilities and environmental risk factors (see reviews in Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009 and Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009). Similar to other diathesis–stress models of psychiatric illness, developmental theories of BPD (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009) based in the biosocial model (Linehan, Reference Linehan1993) argue that biological vulnerabilities, such as impulsivity, interact with environmental effects, such as coercive and invalidating environments, to produce increasingly dysregulated emotional responses which interact with interpersonal and identity processes to culminate in BPD psychopathology. An increasing number of studies have supported this model by assessing developmental precursors leading to BPD symptoms and diagnostic onset (for a review see Stepp, Lazarus, et al., Reference Stepp, Lazarus and Byrd2016). For instance, there are numerous studies demonstrating associations between impulsivity (Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Barkley, Smallish and Fletcher2002; Fossati et al., Reference Fossati, Gratz, Borroni, Maffei, Somma and Carlotta2015; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Flory, Miller, Harty, Newcorn and Helperin2008; O’Grady & Hinshaw, Reference O’Grady and Hinshaw2023; Stepp et al., Reference Stepp, Burke, Hipwell and Loeber2012), emotional sensitivity and reactivity (Stepp, Scott, et al., Reference Stepp, Scott, Jones, Whalen and Hipwell2016), and later development of BPD. Additional lines of work have found that developmental stressors, including invalidating caregiving and hostility (Musser et al., Reference Musser, Zalewski, Stepp and Lewis2018), disorganized attachment (Mosquera et al., Reference Mosquera, Gonzalez and Leeds2014), childhood trauma (Bornovalova et al., Reference Bornovalova, Huibregtse, Hicks, Keyes, McGue and Iacono2013), and peer victimization (Winsper et al., Reference Winsper, Hall, Strauss and Wolke2017) also correspond to later BPD symptoms and diagnosis. Despite this burgeoning empirical literature, reviews and meta-analyses repeatedly indicate that these developmental precursors are largely nonspecific (Bozzatello et al., Reference Bozzatello, Rocca and Bellino2020; Gunderson et al., Reference Gunderson, Herpertz, Skodol, Torgersen and Zanarini2018; Hutsebaut & Aleva, Reference Hutsebaut and Aleva2021; Stepp, Lazarus, et al., Reference Stepp, Lazarus and Byrd2016; Stepp & Lazarus, Reference Stepp and Lazarus2017; Winsper et al., Reference Winsper, Hall, Strauss and Wolke2017).

Thus, in the current paper, we focus on multifinality by examining shared and unique aspects of trajectories of emotion dysregulation as precursors of BPD, depression, and/or externalizing pathology (specifically conduct disorder). Emotion dysregulation has been extensively phenotyped across psychiatric presentations (Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Klein, Crowell, Derbidge and Gatzke-Kopp2009; Beauchaine, Reference Beauchaine2015; Beauchaine & Zisner, Reference Beauchaine and Zisner2017; Chapman, Reference Chapman2019; Crowell & Kaufman, Reference Crowell and Kaufman2016; Gross & Jazaieri, Reference Gross and Jazaieri2014; Vlisides-Henry et al., Reference Vlisides-Henry, Gao, Thomas, Kaliush, Conradt and Crowell2021). However, emotion dysregulation is theorized to be a core and central contributor to the development and maintenance of BPD (Chapman, Reference Chapman2019; Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Putnam & Silk, Reference Putnam and Silk2005). While disturbances in self and social processing are also critical to BPD development, emotional dysregulation with intense expressions of distress is the most widely studied feature (for review see Chapman, Reference Chapman2019). In fact, emotion dysregulation is a salient risk factor for nonsuicidal self-harm and suicidality, some of the most consequential and damaging complications of BPD (Anestis et al., Reference Anestis, Bagge, Tull and Joiner2011; Beauchaine et al., Reference Beauchaine, Hinshaw and Bridge2019; Rajappa et al., Reference Rajappa, Gallagher and Miranda2012; Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Thompson, Thomas, Nesi, Bettis, Ransford, Scopelliti, Frazier and Liu2019). In adults, research and clinical observations support a relationship between problems with general difficulties in emotion regulation and BPD (Kuo & Linehan, Reference Kuo and Linehan2009), as well as specific relationships between BPD and poor distress tolerance (Gratz et al., Reference Gratz, Rosenthal, Tull, Lejuez and Gunderson2006), higher emotional avoidance (Beblo et al., Reference Beblo, Fernando, Kamper, Griepenstroh, Aschenbrenner, Pastuszak, Schlosser and Driessen2013), as well as lower emotional awareness (Leible & Snell, Reference Leible and Snell2004).

In the biosocial model of BPD development, impulsivity and emotional lability interact with environmental factors, leading to increasingly dysregulated emotional expressions. In keeping with this model, difficulties in emotion regulation in young adults partially mediate the relationship between affective intensity and borderline symptoms (Salsman & Linehan, Reference Salsman and Linehan2012). Difficulties with emotion regulation also partially mediate the relationship between childhood trauma and BPD symptoms (Kuo et al., Reference Kuo, Khoury, Metcalfe, Fitzpatrick and Goodwill2015; Peng et al., Reference Peng, Liu, Liu, Chu, Zheng, Wang, Wei, Zhong, Ling and Yi2021). More specifically, the relationship between retrospective assessments of negative familial emotional interactions or parental coercion and BPD symptoms was mediated by emotion regulation (Hope & Chapman, Reference Hope and Chapman2019; Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Puzia, Mead, Crowell, McEachern and Beauchaine2017). When coercive and invalidating parenting is directly assessed via observation, maternal invalidation increased children’s emotional expressions of anger in those with BPD traits (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Baucom, McCauley, Potapova, Fitelson, Barth, Smith and Beauchaine2013). Subsequent work demonstrated this is a transactional process in dyads with self-injuring youth, where their dysregulation increased maternal hostility which then increased adolescent anger (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Butner, Wiltshire, Munion, Yaptangco and Beauchaine2017).

However, despite their utility, these studies have either relied on retrospective reports of environmental effects or the analysis of cross-sectional assessments in environmental interactions, emotion dysregulation, and BPD symptoms. In a prospective, longitudinal study of emotion processing in children with BPD symptoms, the baseline severity of BPD symptoms predicted emotion dysregulation, and the severity and trajectory of emotion dysregulation then predicted subsequent BPD symptoms; the trajectory of emotion dysregulation mediated the relationship between baseline and BPD symptoms at later follow-up (Stepp, Scott, et al., Reference Stepp, Scott, Morse, Nolf, Hallquist and Pilkonis2014). In work by Carlson et al., not only did early childhood activity level and caregiver factors relate to BPD symptoms in adulthood, but middle childhood behavioral and emotion instability, indicators of emotion dysregulation, did as well (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Egeland and Sroufe2009). Such work emphasizes the longitudinal nature of these relationships and the importance of studying the developmental unfolding of emotion dysregulation prospectively and before disorder onset.

Originating in childhood, BPD can be conceptualized via multifinality: early, nonspecific risk factors such as impulsivity and emotional lability interact with environmental factors to influence the trajectory of emotion regulation/dysregulation across development. Thus, the overall trajectory of emotion regulation (and dysregulation) plays a key role in the development and persistence of BPD symptomatology. However, current data is largely limited to assessing these relationships cross-sectionally or using retrospective assessments. The entirety of childhood, and particularly the early developmental period, is rarely considered and/or measured. For instance, our own prior work shows BPD precursors can be identified as early as preschool (Geselowitz et al., Reference Geselowitz, Whalen, Tillman, Barch, Luby and Vogel2020), and prior work by Carlson et al., has found relationships between both child and family factors in infancy and childhood with adult BPD symptoms (Carlson et al., Reference Carlson, Egeland and Sroufe2009). In our prior work, we demonstrated unique relationships between preschool internalizing and externalizing symptoms, low levels of observed maternal support, preschool adverse childhood experiences, and adolescent BPD symptoms (Geselowitz et al., Reference Geselowitz, Whalen, Tillman, Barch, Luby and Vogel2020). Thus, the trajectory of risk for BPD should be assessed across childhood, even from the youngest ages. In the current paper, using our 17-year longitudinal study, we assess how the trajectory of emotion regulation from childhood through adolescence impacts the onset of adolescent BPD symptoms.

Moreover, prior studies have largely considered emotion regulation or dysregulation as a single entity, grouping negative emotions such as a sadness and anger, as well as adaptive and potentially maladaptive regulation strategies such as inhibition, and maladaptive expressions of emotion. Descriptions of an invalidating environment (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009, Reference Crowell, Baucom, McCauley, Potapova, Fitelson, Barth, Smith and Beauchaine2013; Kuo & Linehan, Reference Kuo and Linehan2009; Musser et al., Reference Musser, Zalewski, Stepp and Lewis2018) specify an environment in which the caregiver eschews or rejects the child’s emotional reactions and intermittently reinforces extreme expressions of negative affect. We hypothesize that children experiencing such an environment may be more likely to both generally inhibit their emotions or have expressional reluctance, as well as have increased dysregulated expressions of emotion. However, we are unaware of other studies assessing these components separately, particularly during childhood before disorder onset. Additionally, grouping emotions can be useful, as there is often a moderately strong relationship between dysregulated expressions of various emotions (Suveg et al., Reference Suveg, Hoffman, Zeman and Thomassin2009). However, children (Waters & Thompson, Reference Waters and Thompson2014) and adolescents (Zimmermann & Iwanski, Reference Zimmermann and Iwanski2014) report using different emotion regulation skills for sadness and anger. Further, the diagnostic criteria for BPD specifically references “inappropriate, intense anger or difficulty controlling anger” (American Psychiatric Association, 2013). Yet, as anger has not been assessed separately from other negative emotions such as sadness, it is unclear if expressions and/or inhibition of anger across development are more closely related to BPD, relative to sadness.

Given the importance of emotion dysregulation in BPD, along with the expected changes in emotion regulation occurring during typical development, studying longitudinal trajectories of emotion regulation can inform our understanding of how BPD develops and perhaps, how this may differentiate the development of BPD from other related disorders such as depression and externalizing pathology. Indeed, depression often precedes the development of BPD (Bohus et al., Reference Bohus, Stoffers-Winterling, Sharp, Krause-Utz, Schmahl and Lieb2021; Stepp, Scott, et al., Reference Stepp, Scott, Jones, Whalen and Hipwell2016; Stepp & Lazarus, Reference Stepp and Lazarus2017). Developmental work has also illuminated risk trajectories for BPD emerging from childhood externalizing pathology (Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009; Hallquist et al., Reference Hallquist, Hipwell and Stepp2015; Lyons-Ruth & Brumariu, Reference Lyons-Ruth and Brumariu2021; Stepp et al., Reference Stepp, Burke, Hipwell and Loeber2012; Stepp, Whalen, et al., Reference Stepp, Whalen, Pedersen, Sharp and Tackett2014; Underwood et al., Reference Underwood, Beron and Rosen2011; Wright et al., Reference Wright, Zalewski, Hallquist, Hipwell and Stepp2016). However, there is little empirical data that has examined developmental trajectories of emotion dysregulation leading to all three outcomes (although see Beeney et al., Reference Beeney, Forbes, Hipwell, Nance, Mattia, Lawless, Banihashemi and Stepp2021 for an excellent example), and it is unclear what aspects of emotion regulation and dysregulation trajectories are unique to BPD versus depression and externalizing pathology, a crucial gap given the unique intervention strategies and prognoses for these disparate outcomes.

Understanding the developmental trajectory of various components of emotion processing may improve our ability to change those trajectories. While there are effective psychotherapeutic interventions for BPD (Linehan et al., Reference Linehan, Comtois, Murray, Brown, Gallop, Heard, Korslund, Tutek, Reynolds and Lindenboim2006), including in adolescents (McCauley et al., Reference McCauley, Berk, Asarnow, Adrian, Cohen, Korslund, Avina, Hughes, Harned, Gallop and Linehan2018), these remain relatively time intensive with limited availability and high cost. Understanding whether there are particular components of dysregulation that are most influential in the development of BPD may help specify treatment targets prior to disorder onset and ensuing impairment. Additionally, interventions are often not initiated until long after the onset of impairing symptoms, given that the average age of first treatment for BPD is 18 (Zanarini et al., Reference Zanarini, Frankenburg, Khera and Bleichmar2001), while the onset of self-injury begins prior to age 18 in as many as two thirds of BPD patients (Zanarini et al., Reference Zanarini, Frankenburg, Ridolfi, Jager-Hyman, Hennen and Gunderson2006). For example, Crowell and colleagues (Reference Crowell, Baucom, McCauley, Potapova, Fitelson, Barth, Smith and Beauchaine2013) found that adolescents (ages 13–17 years) with self-harm scored higher on measures of BPD pathology. The trajectory of BPD symptoms in adolescence also predicts the development of social skills, sexual activity, and self-perception (Wright et al., Reference Wright, Zalewski, Hallquist, Hipwell and Stepp2016). Thus, continuing to improve our ability to identify risk trajectories for BPD earlier in childhood may help direct treatment to the most at-risk youth.

Here, we assess the longitudinal trajectories of emotional processing (regulation and dysregulation) from middle childhood through adolescence in a group of children with early-onset psychopathology, who have exhibited elevated rates of BPD symptoms in late adolescence (Boone et al., Reference Boone, Vogel, Tillman, Wright, Barch, Luby and Whalen2022; Geselowitz et al., Reference Geselowitz, Whalen, Tillman, Barch, Luby and Vogel2020). Not only were these emotional processing measures prospectively gathered, but they include separable scales for emotional coping, dysregulation, and inhibition assessed individually for sadness and anger, along with additional scales measuring emotional reluctance and awareness. This allows us to test: (1) whether the developmental trajectories of emotional processing generally impact BPD development; and (2) whether any relationship is specific to emotional dysregulation, inhibition and reluctance, and more specific to anger, as hypothesized. Additionally, as these children were also at increased risk for depression, we were able to assess if there is a specific relationship between developmental trajectories and BPD versus depression and externalizing pathology.

Methods

Participants

Participants were enrolled in the Preschool Depression Study (PDS), a prospective, longitudinal investigation of preschoolers and their families conducted at the Washington University School of Medicine Early Emotional Development Program that has been extensively described elsewhere (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Gaffrey, Tillman, April and Belden2014). Initially, 306 3–6-year-old children and their caregivers were recruited from primary care clinics and day care centers in the St Louis Metropolitan area oversampling for depression using the Preschool Feelings Checklist (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Heffelfinger, Koenig-McNaught, Brown and Spitznagel2004), a validated measure assessing depressive symptoms in the preschool age. Children with symptoms of disruptive behaviors and typically developing children were recruited as comparison groups. All were invited to participate in up to an additional five assessments including clinical interviews, observational assessments, and behavioral questionnaires. A subset of these children were invited for five neuroimaging scans and additional behavioral assessments at those times. In total, assessments spanned 17 years, and a study summary is depicted in Figure 1. All components of this study were done in accordance with review and approval from the Washington University School of Medicine Institutional Review Board (IRB# 201502094). Participants and caregivers were compensated for their time. At each wave, parents provided informed consent; children provided oral consent for the first eight assessments, written assent at the final two assessments (when participants were ages 13.3–21.1 years) if they were not yet 18 years, and informed consent when reaching age 18 years.

Figure 1. Preschool depression study flowchart.

The present study includes 187 participants who completed at least one middle childhood/adolescent timepoint (assessments 4–10, median age 9.0–18.8 years), which allowed for the calculation of emotion regulation trajectories, and who also completed the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C, Crick et al., Reference Crick, Murray-Close and Woods2005) during assessment 9 or 10 (median age 16.3 and 18.8). Demographic information can be found in Table 1. There were no significant differences in demographics or outcome variables between those children included versus excluded from the analyses (Supplemental Table S1).

Table 1. Participant characteristics (n = 187)

Note. ACES = adverse childhood experiences; BPD = borderline personality disorder; BPFS-C = Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children; CD = conduct disorder; MDD = major depressive disorder. BPFS-C was used to assess BPD symptoms, and putative BPD diagnosis was made via BPFS-C score above the recommended cutoff. MDD and CD symptoms were assessed via KSADS, and diagnosis was made via clinical interview.

Measures

Borderline personality symptoms

BPD symptoms were measured by the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C, Crick et al., Reference Crick, Murray-Close and Woods2005) at assessments 9–10 (median age 16.3–18.8). The BPFS-C is a self-report measure with established construct validity (Crick et al., Reference Crick, Murray-Close and Woods2005) and high criterion validity (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Sharp and Ha2011). Scores range from 24 to 120, with higher scores indicating more BPD symptoms; a score greater than 65 is considered the clinical cutoff for a presumptive BPD diagnosis (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Sharp and Ha2011). Scores on the BPFS-C converge with interview-based measures of BPD during adolescence (Chang et al., Reference Chang, Sharp and Ha2011). For participants who had measures at both timepoints, a mean score was calculated.

Emotion processing scales

Emotion regulation was measured by the Child Emotional Management Scales-Anger and -Sadness (CEMS-A and CEMS-S, (Zeman et al., Reference Zeman, Shipman and Penza-Clyve2001, Reference Zeman, Shipman and Suveg2002), collected at assessments 4–10 (median age 9.0–18.8). The CEMS-A and CEMS-S are widely used, validated, and reliable self- and parent-report measures of children’s emotion regulation strategies in response to anger and sadness. Here, we utilize the self-report measures. The CEMS-A and CEMS-S have been shown to be related to other measures of emotion regulation (Zeman et al., Reference Zeman, Shipman and Penza-Clyve2001) and symptoms of psychopathology (Zeman et al., Reference Zeman, Shipman and Suveg2002). Recent work confirms the original three-factor structure (coping, dysregulation, and inhibition) in a diverse, psychiatric sample (Ogbaselase et al., Reference Ogbaselase, Scelsa, Stoppelbein, Becker, Fite, Greening and Luebbe2022). The dysregulation and inhibition subscales, which assess use of maladaptive coping skills, as well as the coping subscale, which assesses use of adaptive coping skills, were all considered separately.

The Emotion Expression Scale for Children (EESC, Penza-Clyve & Zeman, Reference Penza-Clyve and Zeman2002), a self-report questionnaire with high internal consistency and moderate test–retest reliability that examines lack of emotion awareness and lack of motivation to express negative emotion (Penza-Clyve & Zeman, Reference Penza-Clyve and Zeman2002), was administered at assessments 4–8 (median age 9.0–13.4). Here, we included both the poor awareness and expressive reluctance subscales. There are moderate and significant correlations between many of the baseline emotion processing assessments (Table 2), however the strongest correlation was 0.672 between the two EESC subscales, supporting our decision to analyze the effect of each emotion processing scale independently.

Table 2. Correlations of CEMS and EESC subscales at age 10 (N = 137)

Psychiatric symptoms

The core symptoms of MDD and diagnosis were measured via the Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment (PAPA, Egger & Angold, Reference Egger and Angold2004) from baseline to assessment 3, the Childhood and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment (CAPA, Angold & Costello, Reference Angold and Costello2000) at assessments 4–8, and the Kiddie Schedule for Affective Diagnosis and Schizophrenia (KSADS, Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Axelson, Perepletchikova, Brent and Ryan2016) at assessments 9–10. Conduct disorder (CD) symptoms and diagnosis at assessments 9–10 was measured via KSADS (Kaufman et al., Reference Kaufman, Birmaher, Axelson, Perepletchikova, Brent and Ryan2016).

Adverse childhood experiences

The Z-transformed Adverse Childhood Experiences Score (ACES-Z) was based on items identified by Felitti et al. (Reference Felitti, Anda, Nordenberg, Williamson, Spitz, Edwards, Koss and Marks1998), summing items assessing parent-reported poverty, parent-reported parental suicide attempts, substance abuse, and psychopathology from the Family Interview for Genetic Studies (FIGS), and parent- or child-reported traumatic events from the PAPA (assessments 1–3), CAPA (assessments 4–8), and Life Events Checklist (assessments 9–10). This total was converted to a Z-score at each wave and averaged across waves during the preschool period (age 3.0–5.11) and overall (assessments 1–10), as detailed elsewhere (Barch et al., Reference Barch, Belden, Tillman, Whalen and Luby2018).

Questionnaire completion and informants

Table 3 provides details about the number of participants completing each of the measures and participant age by wave. For all caregiver-reported measures (e.g., the psychiatric interviews and FIGS), we obtained information from the family’s identified caregiver. In 94% of cases, the primary caregiver identified was the child participant’s mother.

Table 3. Participant ages and number of participants completing each measure at each study wave

Note. BPFS-C = Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children; CAPA = Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Assessment; CEMS = Child Emotional Management Scale; EESC = Emotion Expression Scale for Children; KSADS = Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia; PAPA = Preschool Age Psychiatric Assessment.

Analyses

Multilevel models (MLM’s) were used to investigate the association of BPFS-C scores (as an independent variable) with each of the coping, dysregulated, and inhibited expression subscales of the CEMS-A and -S, as well as the poor awareness and expressive reluctance subscales of the EESC (in individual models as the dependent variable). Each model included the effect of time as measured by years after the first study wave at which CEMS or EESC was administered, the participant’s age at that first wave, ACES-Z as assessed as above, sex as reported by parent at baseline, family history of affective diagnosis, and BPFS-C score. To assess whether linear or nonlinear models provided the best fit for the data, models were run with time, time-squared, and time-cubed effects. For those models in which there were significant quadratic and/or cubic effects, likelihood ratio tests were conducted to determine which of the nested models provided the best fit, and that model was chosen. In such models, the independent variables, including BPFS-C, could influence not only the overall level of emotion processing (i.e., have the same trajectory shape and slope, just moved up or down from the average), but may also alter the trajectory of emotion processing (i.e., have a steeper slope in emotion processing development at higher rates of BPD symptoms). To assess whether such interactive effects were present and BPD symptoms explained variance in the trajectories of emotion processing measures over time, we included a BPFS-C × time interaction, which was retained in the final model whenever significant. Given that we defined 8 different MLMs, results were corrected for multiple comparisons using the false discovery rate (FDR, Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995). Specifically, all models were run including the BPFS-C × time interaction, and the interaction p-values were FDR corrected. Then, the interaction was removed from all models, and the main effect of BPFS-C was FDR corrected.

When individual items on the CEMS or EESC measures were missing, subscales were calculated by replacing the missing value with the mean value for that item among age- and gender-matched subjects. Therefore, there were no subjects with missing CEMS or EESC subscale scores at waves these measures were completed. Only participants who completed the BPFS-C at assessment 9 and/or 10 and had nonmissing data for the other variables included in the models were included in the analyses. MLM’s allow for missing assessment waves but require full data at each completed wave, so only waves at which the CEMS or EESC were completed contributed to the model, and participants with at least 1 wave of data were included.

Similar MLM’s substituting categorical BPD (defined as BPFS-C > 65 at assessment 9 or 10) for continuous BPFS-C score, were performed, also controlling for sex, ACES-Z, and lifetime MDD diagnosis. These models were also corrected for multiple comparisons (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995) and are reported in the supplement.

Last, we assessed the specificity of these developmental trajectories of emotion regulation to the development of BPD symptoms relative to symptoms of depression and CD, as justified in the introduction. Similar MLM analyses assessing the relationships between emotion regulation measures and symptoms of MDD and CD were performed. Results were corrected for multiple comparisons using FDR (Benjamini & Hochberg, Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995).

All analyses were conducted using SAS v9.4.

Results

Models assessing the developmental trajectories of coping with negative emotions, dysregulated expressions of negative emotions, and inhibition of negative emotions including anger and sadness and their relationship to BPD are shown in Table 4. We detail each of the findings below. To quantify the multifinal effects of these development trajectories of emotion processing, we assessed their relationships to MDD and CD as well (Table 5), with specific relationships discussed below.

Table 4. Multilevel models assessing the effect of BPD symptoms on emotional processing

Note. ACES-Z = adverse childhood experiences Z-scored; BPD = borderline personality disorder; DV = dependent variable; FDR = false discovery rate.

Table 5. Effects of multi-finial psychopathology on trajectories of emotion processing

Note. BPD = borderline personality disorder; CD = conduct disorder; DV = dependent variable; FDR = false discovery rate; IV = independent variable; MDD = major depressive disorder. The effect of each set of symptoms at the final two timepoints, including BPD symptoms as measured by the BPFS-C, MDD symptoms as measured using the KSADS, and CD symptoms, as measured using the KSADS on the trajectories of emotional processing including coping as measured by the CEMS-anger and -sadness coping scales, dysregulation as measured by the CEMS-anger and -sadness dysregulation scales, inhibition as measured by the CEMS-anger and -sadness inhibition scales, and poor emotional awareness and expressive reluctance as measured by the EESC. In models where the interaction between the symptom score and time was significant, these results are also included. Models covaried for age at first wave, sex, and lifetime adverse childhood experiences Z-score.

Emotion coping

As expected, there is an increase in self-ratings of both anger and sadness coping over time, which are both best described by linear models, and there was also a significant effect of age at study entry (Estimate = 0.19, SE = 0.07, p = 0.0063) and female sex (Estimate = −0.55, SE = 0.16, p = 0.0008) on sadness coping trajectory.

Notably, there is an interaction between BPD symptoms and time explaining a small but significant proportion of the variance in the trajectory of both anger (Estimate = −0.005, SE = 0.001, FDR p = 0.0036) and sadness (Estimate = −0.008, SE = 0.001, FDR p < 0.0001) coping scores, indicating different changes in trajectories of coping over time at different levels of BPD symptoms. Graphs of estimated trajectories demonstrate that those with more BPD symptoms have a slower increase in coping over time than those with fewer BPD symptoms (Figure 2). In addition to differences in trajectories of anger coping over time by number of BPD symptoms, there is also a significant difference in anger coping scores at the first wave (Estimate = −0.038, SE = 0.010, p = 0.0001), indicating decreased anger coping in those with more symptoms of BPD.

Figure 2. Estimated trajectories of emotion coping over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Trajectories were assessed via CEMS-sadness and -anger self-report scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

When late adolescent MDD and CD symptoms were substituted for BPD symptoms in separate models, these also had significant effects (Table 5). There was a significant effect of the interaction between MDD symptoms and time on coping with sadness (Estimate = −0.037, SE = 0.009, FDR p < 0.0001) and a marginally significant effect with anger (Estimate = −0.021, SE = 0.009, FDR p = 0.0523). However, when MDD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model with covariates, BPD symptoms were independently associated with coping symptoms while MDD symptoms were not (Sadness coping- BPD × time: Estimate = −0.008, SE = 0.001, p < 0.0001, MDD: Estimate = −0.002, SE = 0.046, p = 0.9590; Anger coping- BPD × time: Estimate = −0.005, SE = 0.001, p = 0.0007, MDD: Estimate = 0.009, SE = 0.049, p = 0.8500). The interaction between CD symptoms and time also had a significant effect on both sadness (Estimate = −0.079, SE = 0.025, FDR p = 0.0144) and anger (Estimate = −0.071, SE = 0.024, FDR p = 0.0148) coping. However, when CD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model with covariates, BPD symptoms were independently associated with coping symptoms while CD symptoms were not (Sadness coping- BPD × time: Estimate = −0.008, SE = 0.001, p < 0.0001, CD: Estimate = −0.030, SE = 0.116, p = 0.7995; Anger coping- BPD × time: Estimate = −0.005, SE = 0.001, p = 0.0008, CD: Estimate = −0.234, SE = 0.123, p = 0.0583).

Emotion dysregulation

Sadness and anger dysregulation demonstrate a more complicated developmental profile. Quadratic models provided the best fit for sadness and anger dysregulation ratings across time. There was a general decrease in ratings of sadness and anger dysregulation in the first several assessments (after the initial baseline assessment) followed by a subsequent increase at the later assessments.

Similar to the coping trajectories, there is an interaction between BPD symptoms and time explaining a small but significant proportion of the variance in the trajectory of both sadness (Estimate = 0.003, SE = 0.001, FDR p = 0.0234) and anger (Estimate = 0.003, SE = 0.001, FDR p = 0.0040) dysregulation scores, indicating different changes in trajectories of dysregulation over time at different levels of BPD symptoms. Graphs of estimated trajectories demonstrate that those with more BPD symptoms have less decrease in dysregulation immediately following the first wave with a steeper increase over the later waves relative to those with fewer BPD symptoms (Figure 3). In addition to differences in trajectories of anger dysregulation over time by number of BPD symptoms, there is also a significant difference in anger dysregulation scores at the first wave (Estimate = 0.024, SE = 0.007, p = 0.0015), indicating increased anger dysregulation in those with more symptoms of BPD.

Figure 3. Estimated trajectories of emotion dysregulation over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Dysregulation was assessed via the self -eport CEMS-sadness and -anger and dysregulation scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

When late adolescent MDD and CD symptoms were substituted for BPD symptoms in separate models, there was a significant effect of MDD symptoms only on anger dysregulation (Estimate = 0.118, SE = 0.038, FDR p = 0.0096) (Table 5). There was no effect of MDD symptoms or the MDD × time interaction on sadness dysregulation. Similar to the coping models, when MDD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model with covariates, BPD symptoms were independently associated with anger dysregulation while MDD symptoms were not (BPD × time: Estimate = 0.003, SD = 0.001, p = 0.0012, MDD: Estimate = 0.033, SE = 0.038, p = 0.3830). Similar to the MDD models, the interaction between CD symptoms and time was significantly associated with anger dysregulation (Estimate = 0.046, SE = 0.018, FDR p = 0.0317), but there was no significant effect of CD symptoms or the CD × time interaction on sadness dysregulation. When CD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model, BPD symptoms were independently associated with anger dysregulation while CD symptoms were not (BPD × time: 0.003, SE = 0.001, p = 0.0012, CD: Estimate = 0.050, SE = 0.097, p = 0.6028).

Emotion inhibition

Unlike the dysregulation and coping scales, inhibition of anger and sadness differed in their relationship to BPD symptoms. Both anger and sadness increased over time and were best described by linear models. Interestingly, there was no relationship between BPD symptoms and the trajectory of anger inhibition, so the interaction was removed from the final model. However, the interaction of BPD symptoms and time accounted for a significant amount of variance in sadness inhibition (Estimate = 0.005, SE = 0.002, FDR p = 0.0048), with graphs of estimated trajectories demonstrating those with more BPD symptoms having sharper increases in sadness inhibition over time relative to those with fewer BPD symptoms (Figure 4). Additionally, BPD symptoms also account for variance in sadness inhibition at the first wave (Estimate = 0.022, SE = 0.009, p = 0.0168), indicating higher initial rates of sadness inhibition in those with increased BPD symptoms. As with other components of expressions of sadness, female sex also partially accounted for variance in the trajectory (Estimate = −0.539, SE = 0.168, p = 0.0016).

Figure 4. Estimated trajectories of emotion inhibition over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Emotion inhibition was assessed via the self-report CEMS-sadness and -anger scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

When late adolescent MDD and CD symptoms were substituted for BPD symptoms in the model, there was a significant effect of MDD symptoms × time only on sadness inhibition (Estimate = 0.030, SE = 0.010, FDR p = 0.0124) (Table 5). CD symptoms had no significant effect on sadness inhibition. As with BPD symptoms, neither MDD nor CD symptoms had an effect on anger inhibition. Similar to the above, when MDD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model with covariates, BPD symptoms were independently associated with sadness inhibition while MDD symptoms were not (BPD × time: Estimate = 0.005, SE = 0.002, p = 0.0028, MDD: Estimate = 0.063, SE = 0.048, p = 0.1886).

Emotional expression

Quadratic models provided the best fit for both poor emotional awareness and expressive reluctance. There was a decrease in both scores during the first few assessments (after the baseline assessment) that increased again during later study assessments. BPD symptoms again explain small but significant proportions of the variance in both trajectories (poor awareness: Estimate = 0.121, SE = 0.029, FDR p < 0.0001; expressive reluctance: Estimate = 0.087, SE = 0.025, FDR p = 0.0006). Graphs of estimated trajectories demonstrate those with more BPD symptoms had overall higher “poor awareness” scores and overall higher expressive reluctance than those with fewer BPD symptoms (Figure 5). There was an additional effect of age at first assessment on the trajectory of poor emotional awareness scores (Estimate = −0.78, SE = 0.30, p = 0.0113).

Figure 5. Estimated trajectories of poor emotional awareness and expressive reluctance over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Emotional awareness was assessed via the poor awareness subscale of the EESC and expressive reluctance via the EESC subscale of the same name. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

When late adolescent MDD and CD symptoms were substituted for BPD symptoms in separate models, there was a similar effect of MDD symptoms × time on both poor awareness (Estimate = 0.528, SE = 0.180, p = 0.0101) and a main effect of MDD symptoms on expressive reluctance (Estimate = 0.346, SE = 0.153, p = 0.0390) (Table 5). CD symptoms had no significant effect on either. Again, when MDD and BPD symptoms were both included in the same model with covariates, BPD symptoms were independently associated with poor awareness (BPD: Estimate = 0.101, SE = 0.032, p = 0.0018, MDD: Estimate = 0.300, SE = 0.194, p = 0.1235) and expressive reluctance (BPD: Estimate = 0.076, SE = 0.027, p = 0.0062, MDD: Estimate = 0.168, SE = 0.166, p = 0.3144), while MDD symptoms were not.

Discussion

In this group of participants enriched for early childhood psychopathology, we find developmental changes in various facets of negative emotional expression across childhood, which relate to BPD symptoms. However, there are notable differences between the developmental trajectories of coping with negative emotions, the dysregulated expressions of negative emotions, and the inhibition of negative emotions. Moreover, there are interesting differences between the developmental trajectories of anger and sadness in their relationship to BPD (Table 4).

The developmental trajectory of emotion dysregulation is a key aspect of the theoretical pathway to BPD in biosocial models, and prior literature demonstrates that dysregulated expressions of emotions are a key mediator between early markers of risk, such as impulsivity and childhood abuse, and BPD symptoms. Here, we use longitudinal modeling to demonstrate that the trajectories of specific components of emotion regulation from childhood to adolescence are associated with adolescent BPD symptoms. Specifically, we show that increased BPD symptoms correspond with a slower improvement in coping with sadness and anger, greater dysregulated expressions of sadness and anger, a faster increase of inhibition of sadness, and greater emotional reluctance and poor awareness. Interestingly, we found no relationship between the developmental trajectory of anger inhibition and BPD symptoms. More gradual developmental trajectories of coping with negative emotions appears to be transdiagnostic, as these showed similar relationships to late adolescent MDD and CD symptoms. In contrast, there was a relatively specific relationship between the trajectory of sadness dysregulation and BPD symptoms relative to MDD or CD symptoms. We found similar associations between late adolescent BPD and MDD symptoms with anger dysregulation, sadness inhibition, poor emotional awareness and expressive reluctance, however the variance explained by MDD appeared to be explained by co-occurring BPD symptoms, as MDD was no longer significant when both were in the model. While there are sex differences, particularly in development of sadness regulation, these sex effects do not explain the observed relationship between these trajectories and BPD.

Self-regulation and related emotion regulation commonly increase across development, are related to improved global functioning, and are inversely correlated with various forms of psychopathology (Nigg, Reference Nigg2017). Consistent with the idea that emotion regulation increases across development and prior work (Zimmermann & Iwanski, Reference Zimmermann and Iwanski2014), we saw a linear increase in coping with both anger and sadness over time, even in our high-risk sample. When assessing emotion regulation strategies, some have been identified as being maladaptive, such as inhibition or suppression, while others have been thought of as generally more adaptive, such as acceptance, reappraisal, and problem solving (for a review see Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010). The Children’s Emotional Management Scale used here largely measures “effective” regulation via the coping subscale. Given the extensive literature demonstrating a relationship between use of adaptive emotion regulation and decreased rates of psychopathology transdiagnostically (see Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010 for a meta-analysis), it is perhaps unsurprising that the longitudinal trajectory of coping with anger and sadness from childhood through adolescence was related to BPD, MDD, and CD symptoms. This is in keeping with prior work demonstrating emotional acceptance is associated with fewer depressive symptoms in adults (Aldao et al., Reference Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema and Schweizer2010) and with possibly fewer depressive symptoms and aggression in adolescents (Price et al., Reference Price, Scelsa, Zeman and Luebbe2022).

In contrast to the coping subscales of the CEMS, the dysregulation subscale items index dysregulated, or maladaptive, expressions of emotions. Interestingly, we found a quadratic model provided the best fit for these trajectories, where there were more dysregulated emotional expressions earlier in childhood and later in adolescence, with a decrease in between. While this may be particular to our participant group, given they were recruited for exhibiting early-onset psychopathology, we are unaware of prior studies assessing emotion dysregulation across such a wide age span with repeated assessments that allow for modeling nonlinear effects. Overall, our findings indicate that increased dysregulated expressions of negative emotions are related to increased BPD symptoms. This is consistent with a prior longitudinal study of emotion dysregulation trajectories in BPD development (Stepp, Scott, et al., Reference Stepp, Scott, Morse, Nolf, Hallquist and Pilkonis2014), which also showed that both the intercept and slope of dysregulated expressions of emotion across adolescence predicted later symptoms of BPD. Our study extends these results to a younger participant age, as our assessments of emotion dysregulation began during middle childhood. Given the emphasis on dysregulated expressions of anger in the diagnostic criteria of BPD, we hypothesized that the developmental trajectory of anger dysregulation would be specifically related to BPD. Contrary to this hypothesis, we found that while there was a relationship between both the trajectory of anger dysregulation (BPFS-C × time interaction) and a difference in initial anger dysregulation with BPD symptoms, this was not specific to BPD. Rather, MDD symptoms also corresponded to an overall difference in anger dysregulation. In contrast, only BPD symptoms explained variance in the change in sadness dysregulation across time. While this was contrary to our hypothesis, it is in keeping with prior work by Zeman et al. (Reference Zeman, Shipman and Suveg2002), who showed that even when assessed separately, dysregulated expressions of both anger and sadness predicted internalizing symptoms, while dysregulated sadness was related to externalizing symptoms. Together, these findings emphasize the importance of both internalizing and externalizing components of BPD.

Prior work finds that individuals with BPD are more likely to suppress (Salsman & Linehan, Reference Salsman and Linehan2012) or avoid negative emotions (Chapman et al., Reference Chapman, Rosenthal and Leung2009; Rosenthal et al., Reference Rosenthal, Cheavens, Lejuez and Lynch2005), which is in keeping with one of the hypothesized means by which invalidating parenting can impact the development of emotion regulation in BPD. As families reject the child’s emotional expressions, this rejection or suppression may then become internalized. Here, we demonstrate that expressive reluctance as assessed via the EESC is related to increased BPD symptoms over time. Additionally, while there is an increase in inhibition of both sadness and anger across childhood, there is little difference in the inhibition of anger related to BPD, MDD, or CD symptoms. Rather, elevated BPD symptoms seem related to both increased initial sadness inhibition and a steeper increase across childhood and adolescence. Again, while neither expressive reluctance nor sadness inhibition was specific to BPD, in that MDD symptoms × time also explained some of the variance in sadness inhibition and MDD symptoms explained variance in expressive reluctance, the variance accounted for by MDD symptoms appears to be primarily due to the overlap with BPD symptoms, as MDD symptoms were no longer significant when both were included in the same model.

Finally, we found poor emotional awareness to be related to BPD symptoms. While this is not specified in developmental models of BPD, there is evidence that effective emotion regulation is dependent on emotional awareness or clarity (Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004), which then acts as a transdiagnostic risk factor for psychopathology (Barrett et al., Reference Barrett, Gross, Christensen and Benvenuto2001; Kranzler et al., Reference Kranzler, Young, Hankin, Abela, Elias and Selby2016). Several clinical theories posit that emotion dysregulation seen in BPD stems from a lack of emotional awareness (Ebner-Priemer et al., Reference Ebner-Priemer, Houben, Santangelo, Kleindienst, Tuerlinckx, Oravecz, Verleysen, Van Deun, Bohus and Kuppens2015; Gratz & Roemer, Reference Gratz and Roemer2004; M. Linehan, Reference Linehan1993). While the quadratic trajectory of awareness over development was unexpected, given that it would appear to depend on underlying processes of internal awareness that should increase with age, it is in keeping with prior work from Kranzler et al. (Reference Kranzler, Young, Hankin, Abela, Elias and Selby2016). The authors found that in childhood and early adolescence emotional awareness was negatively associated with age, and hypothesized a possible explanation that as children move through adolescence there are increasing cognitive and social demands, perhaps taxing their emotion processing resources and decreasing emotional clarity. Such an explanation fits with the trajectory identified here and the similar nonlinear trajectory found for dysregulated expressions of emotion. The transdiagnostic nature of poor awareness is supported by our findings of a similar relationship with MDD symptoms, though again, this relationship appears at least in part due to the covariance with BPD symptoms. However, the independent relationship with BPD symptoms and prior work demonstrating a relationship between poor emotional awareness/alexithymia and self-harm and suicide ideation and behavior (Hemming et al., Reference Hemming, Taylor, Haddock, Shaw and Pratt2019; Iskric et al., Reference Iskric, Ceniti, Bergmans, McInerney and Rizvi2020; Wolff et al., Reference Wolff, Thompson, Thomas, Nesi, Bettis, Ransford, Scopelliti, Frazier and Liu2019), indicate further research into emotional awareness in the development of BPD specifically is important.

Indeed, a recent meta-analysis of 39 studies found a moderate, positive relationship between low emotional awareness (one component of alexithymia) and BPD (Derks et al., Reference Derks, Westerhof and Bohlmeijer2017), and other recent reviews indicate a strong relationship between alexithymia and suicide ideation and behavior and depression(Hemming et al., Reference Hemming, Taylor, Haddock, Shaw and Pratt2019). However, few of the studies reviewed incorporated a clinical control group, and even less included a longitudinal design. Here, we demonstrate a longitudinal, developmental relationship between low emotional awareness and BPD symptoms: poor emotional awareness across childhood and into adolescence was associated with BPD (and to a lesser extent MDD) symptoms in adolescence. Additional work indicates that alexithymia may be a crucial mediator of the relationships between childhood adversity and BPD, as well as childhood adversity and other aspects of emotion dysregulation (Edwards et al., Reference Edwards, Rose, Gromatsky, Feinberg, Kimhy, Doucette, Goodman, McClure, Perez-Rodriguez, New and Hazlett2021), however more longitudinal, prospective work is needed to clarify these relationships across development.

While our study has a number of particular advantages for answering the questions posed, it is not without its limitations. This prospective, 17 year longitudinal study that followed children from preschool through late adolescence has a rich database with not only repeated clinical diagnostic interviews but also multiple measures of emotional processing (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Gaffrey, Tillman, April and Belden2014). This has allowed us to assess the unfolding trajectory of multiple components of emotion processing across an important developmental time frame and provided the opportunity to assess linear and nonlinear changes as presented here. The high rate of BPD in this group by late adolescence was unexpected, but indicates early psychopathology as a potentially important pathway in the development of BPD, as discussed in our prior work (Geselowitz et al., Reference Geselowitz, Whalen, Tillman, Barch, Luby and Vogel2020) and by prospective studies assessing the longitudinal progression of ADHD to BPD and other personality diagnoses (Fischer et al., Reference Fischer, Barkley, Smallish and Fletcher2002; Miller et al., Reference Miller, Flory, Miller, Harty, Newcorn and Helperin2008; O’Grady & Hinshaw, Reference O’Grady and Hinshaw2023). Asking questions about the development of BPD while accounting for the presence of other forms of psychopathology is both a strength and limitation in our study. Given the specific study population, we cannot generalize these findings to development more broadly and may not be able to generalize these findings to other developmental pathways for BPD, such as impulsivity or externalizing pathways. Moreover, given this study was not designed to assess BPD, we do not have specific measures of BPD earlier in development and we are reliant on the BPFS-C, a self-report questionnaire, to identify probable BPD symptoms and diagnosis rather than using a gold standard clinical interview or collateral informants, as was done for the other psychiatric diagnoses assessed here. This also prevents us from seeing transactional relationships between the development of BPD symptoms and changes in emotion processing. While this is a relatively large study group for longitudinal studies of developmental psychopathology including deep phenotyping, it is still a modest size for detecting small effects in nonparametric analyses. While a lack of power seems less likely given the robust effects observed for BPD symptoms, it may contribute to the lack of significant effects seen for MDD and CD symptoms. Moreover, it limits our ability to explore the likely important role of sex differences in the development of emotion processing and how these interact with psychopathology.

Nonetheless, the findings presented here increase our understanding of the developmental trajectories of emotion processing and provide empirical evidence for how variations in these trajectories may impact the development of BPD, MDD, and CD. Identifying children with early-onset psychopathology who are slow to improve their emotion regulation, have ongoing high levels of both sadness and anger dysregulation, attempt to inhibit sadness, and/or have poor emotional awareness may allow us to change these trajectories and prevent or modify eventual BPD symptomatology (along with common co-occurring disorders). This also provides further support for addressing various components of emotion regulation and dysregulation with evidence-based therapies such as parent–child (Luby et al., Reference Luby, Barch, Whalen, Tillman and Freedland2018; Webster-Stratton, Reference Webster-Stratton2005) and dialectical behavioral therapy (DBT) based interventions (M. M. Linehan et al., Reference Linehan, Comtois, Murray, Brown, Gallop, Heard, Korslund, Tutek, Reynolds and Lindenboim2006; McCauley et al., Reference McCauley, Berk, Asarnow, Adrian, Cohen, Korslund, Avina, Hughes, Harned, Gallop and Linehan2018; Perepletchikova et al., Reference Perepletchikova, Nathanson, Axelrod, Merrill, Walker, Grossman, Rebeta, Scahill, Kaufman, Flye, Mauer and Walkup2017).

Overall, our findings highlight the importance of the developmental trajectories of emotion regulation over time in BPD symptoms. We find a particular impact of increased dysregulation in sadness and anger, inhibition of sadness, expressive reluctance, and poor awareness. While there are transdiagnostic effects, in that many of these trajectories also impact late adolescent MDD symptoms, there are also interesting independent effects on BPD symptoms. Together, these findings support the hypothesized role for progression of emotion dysregulation as an important link in biosocial models of BPD (i.e., Crowell et al., Reference Crowell, Beauchaine and Linehan2009) and support addressing emotion regulation in interventions to change the trajectory of BPD symptom development and disorder onset.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579423000627.

Acknowledgements

We wish to thank the children and caregivers for their continued participation in the Preschool Depression Study. All phases of this study were supported by an NIH grant, R01 MH064769-06A1 (PI’s: Luby and Barch). Dr Vogel’s work was supported by NIH grant T32 MH100019-06 and the George and Debra Couch Postdoctoral Scholar Fund. Dr Whalen’s work was supported by NIH grants: K23MH22325028202-01 (PI: Whalen); L30 MH108015 (PI: Whalen). Dr Geselowitz’s work was supported by NIH grant 5 T 35 HL 7815-24.

Competing interests

None.

References

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30(2), 217237. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.) American Psychiatric Association, https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 Google Scholar
Anestis, M. D., Bagge, C. L., Tull, M. T., & Joiner, T. E. (2011). Clarifying the role of emotion dysregulation in the interpersonal-psychological theory of suicidal behavior in an undergraduate sample. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 45(5), 603611. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.10.013 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Angold, A., & Costello, E. J. (2000). The child and adolescent psychiatric assessment (CAPA). Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 39(1), 3948. https://doi.org/10.1097/00004583-200001000-00015 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M., Belden, A. C., Tillman, R., Whalen, D., & Luby, J. L. (2018). Early childhood adverse experiences, inferior frontal gyrus connectivity, and the trajectory of externalizing psychopathology. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 57(3), 183190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.12.011 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F., Gross, J., Christensen, T. C., & Benvenuto, M. (2001). Knowing what you’re feeling and knowing what to do about it: Mapping the relation between emotion differentiation and emotion regulation. Cognition & Emotion, 15(6), 713724. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930143000239 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beauchaine, T. P. (2015). Future directions in emotion dysregulation and youth psychopathology. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 44(5), 875896. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2015.1038827 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Hinshaw, S. P., & Bridge, J. A. (2019). Nonsuicidal self-injury and suicidal behaviors in girls: The case for targeted prevention in preadolescence. Clinical Psychological Science, 7(4), 643667. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702618818474 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., Klein, D. N., Crowell, S. E., Derbidge, C., & Gatzke-Kopp, L. (2009). Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A Biology × Sex × Environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology, 21(3), 735770. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409000418 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beauchaine, T. P., & Zisner, A. (2017). Motivation, emotion regulation, and the latent structure of psychopathology: An integrative and convergent historical perspective. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 119, 108118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.12.014 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beblo, T., Fernando, S., Kamper, P., Griepenstroh, J., Aschenbrenner, S., Pastuszak, A., Schlosser, N., & Driessen, M. (2013). Increased attempts to suppress negative and positive emotions in Borderline Personality Disorder. Psychiatry Research, 210(2), 505509. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2013.06.036 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beeney, J. E., Forbes, E. E., Hipwell, A. E., Nance, M., Mattia, A., Lawless, J. M., Banihashemi, L., & Stepp, S. D. (2021). Determining the key childhood and adolescent risk factors for future BPD symptoms using regularized regression: Comparison to depression and conduct disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(2), 223231. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.13269 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Benjamini, Y., & Hochberg, Y. (1995). Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society: Series B (Methodological), 57(1), 289300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.x Google Scholar
Bohus, M., Stoffers-Winterling, J., Sharp, C., Krause-Utz, A., Schmahl, C., & Lieb, K. (2021). Borderline personality disorder. The Lancet, 398(10310), 15281540. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(21)00476-1 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boone, K., Vogel, A. C., Tillman, R., Wright, A. J., Barch, D. M., Luby, J. L., & Whalen, D. J. (2022). Identifying moderating factors during the preschool period in the development of borderline personality disorder: A prospective longitudinal analysis. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 9(1), 26. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-022-00198-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornovalova, M. A., Huibregtse, B. M., Hicks, B. M., Keyes, M., McGue, M., & Iacono, W. (2013). Tests of a direct effect of childhood abuse on adult borderline personality disorder traits: A longitudinal discordant twin design. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 122(1), 180194. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028328 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bozzatello, P., Rocca, P., & Bellino, S. (2020). Trauma and psychopathology associated with early onset BPD: An empirical contribution. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 131, 5459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2020.08.038 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carlson, E. A., Egeland, B., & Sroufe, L. A. (2009). A prospective investigation of the development of borderline personality symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 21(4), 13111334. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579409990174 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chang, B., Sharp, C., & Ha, C. (2011). The criterion validity of the borderline personality features scale for children in an adolescent inpatient setting. Journal of Personality Disorders, 25(4), 492503. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2011.25.4.492 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chapman, A. L. (2019). Borderline personality disorder and emotion dysregulation. Development and Psychopathology, 31(3), 11431156. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579419000658 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, A. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., & Leung, D. W. (2009). Emotion suppression in borderline personality disorder: An experience sampling study. Journal of Personality Disorders, 23(1), 2947. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2009.23.1.29 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crick, N. R., Murray-Close, D., & Woods, K. (2005). Borderline personality features in childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology, 17(4), 1051–1070. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050492 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Baucom, B. R., McCauley, E., Potapova, N. V., Fitelson, M., Barth, H., Smith, C. J., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2013). Mechanisms of contextual risk for adolescent self-injury: Invalidation and conflict escalation in mother-child interactions. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42(4), 467480. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2013.785360 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Beauchaine, T. P., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending linehan’s theory. Psychological Bulletin, 135(3), 495510. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015616 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., Butner, J. E., Wiltshire, T. J., Munion, A. K., Yaptangco, M., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2017). Evaluating emotional and biological sensitivity to maternal behavior among self-injuring and depressed adolescent girls using nonlinear dynamics. Clinical Psychological Science, 5(2), 272285. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702617692861 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowell, S. E., & Kaufman, E. A. (2016). Development of self-inflicted injury: Comorbidities and continuities with borderline and antisocial personality traits. Development and Psychopathology, 28(4pt1), 10711088. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579416000705 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Derks, Y. P. M. J., Westerhof, G. J., & Bohlmeijer, E. T. (2017). A meta-analysis on the association between emotional awareness and borderline personality pathology. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31(3), 362384. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_257 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ebner-Priemer, U. W., Houben, M., Santangelo, P., Kleindienst, N., Tuerlinckx, F., Oravecz, Z., Verleysen, G., Van Deun, K., Bohus, M., Kuppens, P. (2015). Unraveling affective dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: A theoretical model and empirical evidence. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(1), 186198.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, E. R., Rose, N. L. J., Gromatsky, M., Feinberg, A., Kimhy, D., Doucette, J. T., Goodman, M., McClure, M. M., Perez-Rodriguez, M. M., New, A. S., Hazlett, E. A. (2021). Alexithymia, affective lability, impulsivity, and childhood adversity in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 35(Supplement A), 114131. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2021_35_513 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Egger, H. L., & Angold, A. (2004). The preschool age psychiatric assessment (PAPA): A structured parent interview for diagnosing psychiatric disorders in preschool children. In Handbook of infant, toddler, and preschool mental health assessment (pp. 223243). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Felitti, V. J., Anda, R. F., Nordenberg, D., Williamson, D. F., Spitz, A. M., Edwards, V., Koss, M. P., & Marks, J. S. (1998). Relationship of childhood abuse and household dysfunction to many of the leading causes of death in adults. The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine, 14(4), 245258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2002). Young adult follow-up of hyperactive children: Self-reported psychiatric disorders, comorbidity, and the role of childhood conduct problems and teen CD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 30(5), 463475. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1019864813776 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fossati, A., Gratz, K. L., Borroni, S., Maffei, C., Somma, A., & Carlotta, D. (2015). The relationship between childhood history of ADHD symptoms and DSM-IV borderline personality disorder features among personality disordered outpatients: The moderating role of gender and the mediating roles of emotion dysregulation and impulsivity. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 56, 121127. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.09.023 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geselowitz, B., Whalen, D. J., Tillman, R., Barch, D. M., Luby, J. L., & Vogel, A. (2020). Preschool age predictors of adolescent borderline personality symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 60(5), 612622. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2020.07.908 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gratz, K. L., & Roemer, L. (2004). Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: Development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 26(1), 4154. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gratz, K. L., Rosenthal, M. Z., Tull, M. T., Lejuez, C. W., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). An experimental investigation of emotion dysregulation in borderline personality disorder. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 115(4), 850855. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.850 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grilo, C. M., McGlashan, T. H., Quinlan, D. M., Walker, M. L., Greenfeld, D., & Edell, W. S. (1998). Frequency of personality disorders in two age cohorts of psychiatric inpatients. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(1), 140142. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.155.1.140 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gross, J. J., & Jazaieri, H. (2014). Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: An affective science perspective. Clinical Psychological Science, 2(4), 387401. https://doi.org/10.1177/2167702614536164 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunderson, J. G., Herpertz, S. C., Skodol, A. E., Torgersen, S., & Zanarini, M. C. (2018). Borderline personality disorder. Nature Reviews Disease Primers, 4(1), 18029. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrdp.2018.29 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallquist, M. N., Hipwell, A. E., & Stepp, S. D. (2015). Poor self-control and harsh punishment in childhood prospectively predict borderline personality symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 124(3), 549564. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000058 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hemming, L., Taylor, P., Haddock, G., Shaw, J., & Pratt, D. (2019). A systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between alexithymia and suicide ideation and behaviour. Journal of Affective Disorders, 254, 3448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2019.05.013 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hope, N. H., & Chapman, A. L. (2019). Difficulties regulating emotions mediates the associations of parental psychological control and emotion invalidation with borderline personality features. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 10(3), 267274. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000316 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutsebaut, J., & Aleva, A. (2021). The identification of a risk profile for young people with borderline personality pathology: A review of recent literature. Current Opinion in Psychology, 37, 1320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.06.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iskric, A., Ceniti, A. K., Bergmans, Y., McInerney, S., & Rizvi, S. J. (2020). Alexithymia and self-harm: A review of nonsuicidal self-injury, suicidal ideation, and suicide attempts. Psychiatry Research, 288, 112920. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2020.112920 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, E. A., Puzia, M. E., Mead, H. K., Crowell, S. E., McEachern, A., & Beauchaine, T. P. (2017). Children’s emotion regulation difficulties mediate the association between maternal borderline and antisocial symptoms and youth behavior problems over 1 year. Journal of Personality Disorders, 31(2), 170192. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2016_30_244 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Axelson, D. A., Perepletchikova, F., Brent, D., & Ryan, N. The Kiddie Schedule for Affective Diagnoses and Schizophrenia -Present and Lifetime-for DSM 5 (2016).Google Scholar
Korzekwa, M. I., Dell, P. F., Links, P. S., Thabane, L., & Webb, S. P. (2008). Estimating the prevalence of borderline personality disorder in psychiatric outpatients using a two-phase procedure. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 49(4), 380386. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2008.01.007 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kranzler, A., Young, J. F., Hankin, B. L., Abela, J. R. Z., Elias, M. J., & Selby, E. A. (2016). Emotional awareness: A transdiagnostic predictor of depression and anxiety for children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology, 45(3), 262269. https://doi.org/10.1080/15374416.2014.987379 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, J. R., Khoury, J. E., Metcalfe, R., Fitzpatrick, S., & Goodwill, A. (2015). An examination of the relationship between childhood emotional abuse and borderline personality disorder features: The role of difficulties with emotion regulation. Child Abuse & Neglect, 39, 147155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chiabu.2014.08.008 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuo, J. R., & Linehan, M. M. (2009). Disentangling emotion processes in borderline personality disorder: Physiological and self-reported assessment of biological vulnerability, baseline intensity, and reactivity to emotionally evocative stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118(3), 531544. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0016392 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leible, T. L., & Snell, W. E. (2004). Borderline personality disorder and multiple aspects of emotional intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(2), 393404. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.09.011 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Linehan, M. (1993). Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Linehan, M. M., Comtois, K. A., Murray, A. M., Brown, M. Z., Gallop, R. J., Heard, H. L., Korslund, K. E., Tutek, D. A., Reynolds, S. K., Lindenboim, N. (2006). Two-year randomized controlled trial and follow-up of dialectical behavior therapy vs therapy by experts for suicidal behaviors and borderline personality disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63(7), 757766. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.63.7.757 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Barch, D. M., Whalen, D., Tillman, R., & Freedland, K. E. (2018). A randomized controlled trial of parent-child psychotherapy targeting emotion development for early childhood depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 175(11), 11021110. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2018.18030321 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Gaffrey, M. S., Tillman, R., April, L. M., & Belden, A. C. (2014). Trajectories of preschool disorders to full DSM depression at school age and early adolescence: Continuity of preschool depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 171(7), 768776. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091198 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, J. L., Heffelfinger, A., Koenig-McNaught, A. L., Brown, K., & Spitznagel, E. (2004). The preschool feelings checklist: A brief and sensitive screening measure for depression in young children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(6), 708717. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.chi.0000121066.29744.08 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lyons-Ruth, K., & Brumariu, L. E. (2021). Emerging child competencies and personality pathology: Toward a Developmental Cascade model of BPD. Current Opinion in Psychology, 37, 3238. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.004 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, E., Berk, M. S., Asarnow, J. R., Adrian, M., Cohen, J., Korslund, K., Avina, C., Hughes, J., Harned, M., Gallop, R., Linehan, M. M. (2018). Efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy for adolescents at high risk for suicide: A randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry, 75(8), 777785. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.1109 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, C. J., Flory, J. D., Miller, S. R., Harty, S. C., Newcorn, J. H., & Helperin, J. M. (2008). Childhood attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and the emergence of personality disorders in adolescence: A prospective follow-up study. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 69(9), 14771484. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v69n0916 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mosquera, D., Gonzalez, A., & Leeds, A. M. (2014). Early experience, structural dissociation, and emotional dysregulation in borderline personality disorder: The role of insecure and disorganized attachment. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 1(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1186/2051-6673-1-15 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musser, N., Zalewski, M., Stepp, S., & Lewis, J. (2018). A systematic review of negative parenting practices predicting borderline personality disorder: Are we measuring biosocial theory’s “invalidating environment”? Clinical Psychology Review, 65, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cpr.2018.06.003 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nigg, J. T. (2017). Annual Research Review: On the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 58(4), 361383. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcpp.12675 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Grady, S. M., & Hinshaw, S. P. (2023). Developmental predictors of young adult borderline personality disorder: A prospective, longitudinal study of females with and without childhood ADHD. BMC Psychiatry, 23(1), 106. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888-023-04515-3 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ogbaselase, F. A., Scelsa, V. L., Stoppelbein, L., Becker, S. P., Fite, P. J., Greening, L., & Luebbe, A. M. (2022). Psychometric properties of the children’s emotion management scales within a psychiatric sample. Psychological Assessment, 34(7), 620630. https://doi.org/10.1037/pas0001131 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peng, W., Liu, Z., Liu, Q., Chu, J., Zheng, K., Wang, J., Wei, H., Zhong, M., Ling, Y., Yi, J. (2021). Insecure attachment and maladaptive emotion regulation mediating the relationship between childhood trauma and borderline personality features. Depression and Anxiety, 38(1), 2839. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.23082 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Penza-Clyve, S., & Zeman, J. (2002). Initial validation of the Emotion Expression Scale for Children (EESC). Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 31(4), 540547. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3104_12 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perepletchikova, F., Nathanson, D., Axelrod, S. R., Merrill, C., Walker, A., Grossman, M., Rebeta, J., Scahill, L., Kaufman, J., Flye, B., Mauer, E., Walkup, J. (2017). Randomized clinical trial of dialectical behavior therapy for preadolescent children with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder: Feasibility and outcomes. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 56(10), 832840. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jaac.2017.07.789 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pompili, M., Girardi, P., Ruberto, A., & Tatarelli, R. (2005). Suicide in borderline personality disorder: A meta-analysis. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 59(5), 319324. https://doi.org/10.1080/08039480500320025 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Price, N. N., Scelsa, V. L., Zeman, J. L., & Luebbe, A. M. (2022). Profiles of adolescents’ sadness, anger, and worry regulation: Characterization and relations with psychopathology. Emotion, 23(2), 473485. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0001084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Putnam, K. M., & Silk, K. R. (2005). Emotion dysregulation and the development of borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 17(4), 899–925. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579405050431 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rajappa, K., Gallagher, M., & Miranda, R. (2012). Emotion dysregulation and vulnerability to suicidal ideation and attempts. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 36(6), 833839. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10608-011-9419-2 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosenthal, M. Z., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Lynch, T. R. (2005). Thought suppression mediates the relationship between negative affect and borderline personality disorder symptoms. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43(9), 11731185. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salsman, N. L., & Linehan, M. M. (2012). An investigation of the relationships among negative affect, difficulties in emotion regulation, and features of borderline personality disorder. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 34(2), 260267. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10862-012-9275-8 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stepp, S. D., Burke, J. D., Hipwell, A. E., & Loeber, R. (2012). Trajectories of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and oppositional defiant disorder symptoms as precursors of borderline personality disorder symptoms in adolescent girls. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(1), 720. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10802-011-9530-6 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., & Lazarus, S. A. (2017). Identifying a borderline personality disorder prodrome: Implications for community screening: BPD prodrome for community screening. Personality and Mental Health, 11(3), 195205. https://doi.org/10.1002/pmh.1389 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., Lazarus, S. A., & Byrd, A. L. (2016). A systematic review of risk factors prospectively associated with borderline personality disorder: Taking stock and moving forward. Personality Disorders, 7(4), 316323. https://doi.org/10.1037/per0000186 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., Scott, L. N., Jones, N. P., Whalen, D. J., & Hipwell, A. E. (2016). Negative emotional reactivity as a marker of vulnerability in the development of borderline personality disorder symptoms. Development and Psychopathology, 28(1), 213224. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579415000395 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., Scott, L. N., Morse, J. Q., Nolf, K. A., Hallquist, M. N., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2014). Emotion dysregulation as a maintenance factor of borderline personality disorder features. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 55(3), 657666. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.11.006 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stepp, S. D., Whalen, D. J., & Pedersen, S. L. (2014). The externalizing pathway to borderline personality disorder in youth. In Sharp, C., & Tackett, J. L. (Eds.), Handbook of borderline personality disorder in children and adolescents (pp. 247263), Springer, New York, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-0591-1_17 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suveg, C., Hoffman, B., Zeman, J. L., & Thomassin, K. (2009). Common and specific emotion-related predictors of anxious and depressive symptoms in youth. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40(2), 223239. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10578-008-0121-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(6), 590596. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpsyc.58.6.590 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, M. K., Beron, K. J., & Rosen, L. H. (2011). Joint trajectories for social and physical aggression as predictors of adolescent maladjustment: Internalizing symptoms, rule-breaking behaviors, and borderline and narcissistic personality features. Development and Psychopathology, 23(2), 659678. https://doi.org/10.1017/S095457941100023X CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Asselt, A. D. I., Dirksen, C. D., Arntz, A., & Severens, J. L. (2007). The cost of borderline personality disorder: Societal cost of illness in BPD-patients. European Psychiatry: The Journal of the Association of European Psychiatrists, 22(6), 354361. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.04.001 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vlisides-Henry, R. D., Gao, M., Thomas, L., Kaliush, P. R., Conradt, E., & Crowell, S. E. (2021). Digital phenotyping of emotion dysregulation across lifespan transitions to better understand psychopathology risk. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 618442. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.618442 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waters, S. F., & Thompson, R. A. (2014). Children’s perceptions of the effectiveness of strategies for regulating anger and sadness. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 174181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515410 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Webster-Stratton, C. (2005). The incredible years: A training series for the prevention and treatment of conduct problems in young children, Psychosocial treatments for child and adolescent disorders: Empirically based strategies for clinical practice (2nd ed., pp. 507555). American Psychological Association, https://doi.org/10.1037/10196-000 Google Scholar
Winsper, C., Hall, J., Strauss, V. Y., & Wolke, D. (2017). Aetiological pathways to borderline personality disorder symptoms in early adolescence: Childhood dysregulated behaviour, maladaptive parenting and bully victimisation. Borderline Personality Disorder and Emotion Dysregulation, 4(1), 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40479-017-0060-x CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolff, J. C., Thompson, E., Thomas, S. A., Nesi, J., Bettis, A. H., Ransford, B., Scopelliti, K., Frazier, E. A., & Liu, R. T. (2019). Emotion dysregulation and non-suicidal self-injury: A systematic review and meta-analysis. European Psychiatry, 59, 2536. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.03.004 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wright, A. G. C., Zalewski, M., Hallquist, M. N., Hipwell, A. E., & Stepp, S. D. (2016). Developmental trajectories of borderline personality disorder symptoms and psychosocial functioning in adolescence. Journal of Personality Disorders, 30(3), 351372. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi_2015_29_200 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Khera, G. S., & Bleichmar, J. (2001). Treatment histories of borderline inpatients. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 42(2), 144150. https://doi.org/10.1053/comp.2001.19749 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zanarini, M. C., Frankenburg, F. R., Ridolfi, M. E., Jager-Hyman, S., Hennen, J., & Gunderson, J. G. (2006). Reported childhood onset of self-mutilation among borderline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders, 20(1), 915. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2006.20.1.9 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Penza-Clyve, S. (2001). Development and initial validation of the children’s sadness management scale. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 25(3), 187205. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1010623226626 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zeman, J., Shipman, K., & Suveg, C. (2002). Anger and sadness regulation: Predictions to internalizing and externalizing symptoms in children. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology: The Official Journal for the Society of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, American Psychological Association, Division 53, 31(3), 393398. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15374424JCCP3103_11 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zimmermann, P., & Iwanski, A. (2014). Emotion regulation from early adolescence to emerging adulthood and middle adulthood: Age differences, gender differences, and emotion-specific developmental variations. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 38(2), 182194. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025413515405 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Preschool depression study flowchart.

Figure 1

Table 1. Participant characteristics (n = 187)

Figure 2

Table 2. Correlations of CEMS and EESC subscales at age 10 (N = 137)

Figure 3

Table 3. Participant ages and number of participants completing each measure at each study wave

Figure 4

Table 4. Multilevel models assessing the effect of BPD symptoms on emotional processing

Figure 5

Table 5. Effects of multi-finial psychopathology on trajectories of emotion processing

Figure 6

Figure 2. Estimated trajectories of emotion coping over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Trajectories were assessed via CEMS-sadness and -anger self-report scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

Figure 7

Figure 3. Estimated trajectories of emotion dysregulation over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Dysregulation was assessed via the self -eport CEMS-sadness and -anger and dysregulation scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

Figure 8

Figure 4. Estimated trajectories of emotion inhibition over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Emotion inhibition was assessed via the self-report CEMS-sadness and -anger scales. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

Figure 9

Figure 5. Estimated trajectories of poor emotional awareness and expressive reluctance over time. Note. BPD Sxs = borderline personality disorder symptoms; Emotional awareness was assessed via the poor awareness subscale of the EESC and expressive reluctance via the EESC subscale of the same name. Trajectories are plotted for the mean BPFS-C score as well as those 1 SD above the mean and 1 SD below the mean, shaded by intensity of BPFS-C symptoms.

Supplementary material: File

Vogel et al. supplementary material

Vogel et al. supplementary material

Download Vogel et al. supplementary material(File)
File 33.5 KB