Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T21:43:06.864Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Abstractions, predictions, and speech sound representations

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 June 2020

Mathias Scharinger*
Affiliation:
Phonetics Research Group, Department of German Linguistics, Philipps-University, 35032Marburg, Germany. mathias.scharinger@gmail.comhttp://www.inter-word.net/home/ Center for Mind, Brain & Behavior, 35032MarburgGermany

Abstract

Gilead et al. provide a unified account of predictive cognition in which abstract representations play an essential role. Although acknowledging the similarity to linguistic concepts toward the higher end of the proposed abstraction gradient, Gilead et al. do not consider the potential of their account to embrace phonetic and phonological speech sound representations and their neural bases.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2020. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baldeweg, T. (2006) Repetition effects to sounds: Evidence for predictive coding in the auditory system. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(3):9394.10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. & Halle, M. (1968) The sound pattern of English. Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Clements, G. N. (1985) The geometry of phonological features. Phonology Yearbook 2:225–53.10.1017/S0952675700000440CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friston, K. (2005) A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 360(1456):815–36.10.1098/rstb.2005.1622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gładziejewski, P. (2015) Predictive coding and representationalism. Synthese 124. doi: 10.1007/s11229-015-0762-9.Google Scholar
Gow, D. W. Jr. (2001) Assimilation and anticipation in continuous spoken word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language 45(1):133–59.10.1006/jmla.2000.2764CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Humphries, C., Sabri, M., Lewis, K. & Liebenthal, E. (2014) Hierarchical organization of speech perception in human auditory cortex. Frontiers in Neuroscience 8(406):112.10.3389/fnins.2014.00406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jakobson, R., Fant, G. M. & Halle, M. (1965) Preliminaries to speech analysis – The distinctive features and their correlates. The MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lahiri, A. & Reetz, H. (2010) Distinctive features: Phonological underspecification in representation and processing. Journal of Phonetics 38:4459.10.1016/j.wocn.2010.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Markman, A. B. & Dietrich, E. (2000) In defense of representation. Cognitive Psychology 40(2):138–71.10.1006/cogp.1999.0727CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medin, D. L. (1989) Concepts and conceptual structure. American Psychologist 44(12):1469–81.10.1037/0003-066X.44.12.1469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Möttönen, R., Dutton, R. & Watkins, K. E. (2013) Auditory-motor processing of speech sounds. Cerebral Cortex 23(5):1190–97.10.1093/cercor/bhs110CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Möttönen, R. & Watkins, K. E. (2009) Motor representations of articulators contribute to categorical perception of speech sounds. The Journal of Neuroscience 29(31):9819–25.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6018-08.2009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Obleser, J. & Eisner, F. (2009) Pre-lexical abstraction of speech in the auditory cortex. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13(1):1419.10.1016/j.tics.2008.09.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramsey, W. M. (2010) Representation reconsidered. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rosch, E. (1973) Natural categories. Cognitive Psychology 4(3):328–50.10.1016/0010-0285(73)90017-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosen, D. A. (1975) An argument for the logical notion of a memory trace. Philosophy of Science 42(1):110.10.1086/288614CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scharinger, M., Monahan, P. J. & Idsardi, W. J. (2016) Linguistic category structure influences early auditory processing: Converging evidence from mismatch responses and cortical oscillations. NeuroImage 128:293301.10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.01.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steriade, D. (1995) Underspecification and markedness. In: The handbook of phonological theory, ed. Goldsmith, J. A., pp. 114–74. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stevens, K. N. (2002) Toward a model for lexical access based on acoustic landmarks and distinctive features. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 111:1872–91.10.1121/1.1458026CrossRefGoogle Scholar