Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vfjqv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-25T11:17:48.136Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Cognitive paths from trauma to posttraumatic stress disorder: a prospective study of Ehlers and Clark's model in survivors of assaults or road traffic collisions

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 September 2019

Esther T. Beierl*
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, Oxford, UK
Inga Böllinghaus
Affiliation:
King's College London, London, UK
David M. Clark
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, Oxford, UK King's College London, London, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
Edward Glucksman
Affiliation:
King's College Hospital NHS Foundation Trust, London, UK
Anke Ehlers
Affiliation:
University of Oxford, Oxford, UK King's College London, London, UK Oxford Health NHS Foundation Trust, Oxford, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Esther T. Beierl, E-mail: esther.beierl@psy.ox.ac.uk; Anke Ehlers, E-mail: anke.ehlers@psy.ox.ac.uk
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Background

Individual differences in cognitive responses to trauma may represent modifiable risk factors that could allow early identification, targeted early treatment and possibly prevention of chronic posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Ehlers and Clark's cognitive model of PTSD suggests that negative appraisals, disjointed trauma memories, and unhelpful coping strategies maintain PTSD. These are thought to be influenced by cognitive processing during trauma. The aim of this study was to test this model prospectively with path analyses.

Methods

Participants (N = 828) were recruited from an emergency department following injury in a violent assault or road traffic collision and 700 participated in the 6-month assessments. Cognitive processing was assessed shortly after the event, negative appraisals, disjointed memories, and unhelpful coping strategies at 1 month, persistent PTSD symptom severity at 6 months, and early PTSD symptom severity at 2 weeks.

Results

Cognitive variables, with trauma type and gender, explained 52% of the variance in PTSD symptom severity at 6 months. Including early symptom severity in the model did not explain more variance (53%). Early PTSD symptom severity, with trauma type and gender, only predicted 40%. Negative appraisals and disjointed memories predicted persistent symptom severity both directly and indirectly via unhelpful strategies. Peritraumatic processing predicted persistent symptom severity mainly indirectly. The effects of trauma type and gender were fully mediated by the cognitive factors.

Conclusions

The results are consistent with theoretically derived predictions and support cognitive factors as indicators of risk for chronic PTSD and as a target for the treatment and prevention of PTSD.

Type
Original Articles
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s) 2019

Introduction

Although most people are exposed to one or more traumatic events during their life time (Benjet et al., Reference Benjet, Bromet, Karam, Kessler, McLaughlin, Ruscio, Shahly, Stein, Petukhova, Hill, Alonso, Atwoli, Bunting, Bruffaerts, Caldas-de-Almeida, de Girolamo, Florescu, Gureje, Huang, Lepine, Kawakami, Kovess-Masfety, Medina-Mora, Navarro-Mateu, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Scott, Shalev, Slade, ten Have, Torres, Viana, Zarkov and Koenen2016; Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso, Degenhardt, de Girolamo, Dinolova, Ferry, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Huang, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lepine, Levinson, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Scott, Stein, ten Have, Torres, Viana, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Koenen2017), only a minority develop PTSD, of whom nearly half recover without treatment during the following months (Morina et al., Reference Morina, Wicherts, Lobbrecht and Priebe2014). This raises the question of how to best identify the significant minority of individuals who develop chronic PTSD. Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model of PTSD (see Fig. 1) was developed to explain why some people do not recover after trauma and to identify maintaining factors that can be modified in treatment. The model suggests that chronic PTSD develops if trauma survivors process the traumatic event in a way that poses a serious current threat. The perceived threat has two sources: First, people with chronic PTSD show excessively negative appraisals (personal meanings) of the trauma and/or its sequelae. These appraisals are idiosyncratic and commonly include both appraisals about the self, such as ‘I am inadequate', and appraisals of other people/the world, e.g. ‘People cannot be trusted’. Second, the trauma memory is disjointed and poorly elaborated. Together with high perceptual priming and strong associative learning for stimuli present during the trauma, this leads to easy cue-driven triggering of memories (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Michael, Chen, Payne and Shan2006, Reference Ehlers, Sündermann, Boellinghaus, Vossbeck-Elsebusch, Gamer, Briddon, Martin and Glucksman2010; for a review see Marks et al., Reference Marks, Franklin and Zoellner2018). These memories are retrieved without a context and retain the original highly threatening meanings, as they are poorly linked with other information in memory that would put them into perspective (e.g. ‘I did not die’; Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). Memory characteristics and negative appraisals are assumed to have a reciprocal relationship.

Fig. 1. Conceptual figure of cognitive factors leading to chronic PTSD according to Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model.

Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model further proposes that the sense of current threat motivates a series of behavioural and cognitive strategies that are intended to reduce the current threat but maintain the symptoms. These strategies include excessive precautions (safety behaviours), maladaptive responses to intrusive memories (thought suppression, rumination, intentional numbing), and persistent dissociation.

Thus, the core hypothesis of Ehlers and Clark's model is that appraisals, disjointed memories, and unhelpful strategies maintain PTSD. Several studies have found that these cognitive factors predict chronic PTSD (appraisals: e.g. Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Bryant and Guthrie, Reference Bryant and Guthrie2005; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008; Lancaster et al., Reference Lancaster, Rodriguez and Weston2011; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013; for a review see Brown et al., Reference Brown, Belli, Asnaani and Foa2019; disjointed memories: e.g. Murray et al., Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013; safety behaviours: Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008, responses to intrusions: Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant1998; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a, Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013; persistent dissociation: Murray et al., Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002; Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a).

Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model further hypothesises that cognitive processing during the trauma predicts the onset of PTSD symptoms as they influence appraisals and memory characteristics. Four styles of problematic processing are specified: Data-driven processing refers to a preferential processing of perceptual information during the trauma (as opposed to its meaning) and is thought to increase the disjointedness of trauma memories and perceptual priming. Lack of self-referent processing (i.e. an inability of processing the experience as part of the continuum of autobiographical experiences) and dissociation (derealisation and depersonalisation) are also thought to contribute to the disjointedness of trauma memories. Mental defeat (i.e. a perceived loss of all autonomy during trauma), and lack of self-referent processing are thought to facilitate appraisals about a negative view of oneself and perceived permanent change. Several studies showed that these patterns of cognitive processing predict PTSD (e.g. data-driven processing: Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013; dissociation: Ozer et al., Reference Ozer, Best, Lipsey and Weiss2003; lack of self-referent processing: Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a; mental defeat: Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a).

The aim of the current study was to test prospectively the direct and indirect paths by which the cognitive factors specified in Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model lead to persistent PTSD symptoms at 6 months after trauma. While earlier studies provided evidence that subsets of these cognitive variables predict chronic PTSD (e.g. Lancaster et al., Reference Lancaster, Rodriguez and Weston2011; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a) and also do so when controlling for early symptom severity (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant1998; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2007; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008), the present study used a more comprehensive set of measures and had a larger sample size that allowed testing the serial mediation effects proposed by Ehlers and Clark (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000). As a first step, we tested the central model of the maintenance of PTSD and hypothesized that negative appraisals and disjointed memories are correlated, and each predict persistent PTSD symptom severity directly and indirectly via unhelpful strategies (safety behaviours, responses to intrusions, and persistent dissociation). In the second step, we included the hypothesized role of cognitive processing styles during trauma in influencing appraisals and memory characteristics. We predicted that these are correlated, and each indirectly predicts PTSD symptom severity at 6 months, via different pathways: data-driven processing and dissociation via greater disjointedness of trauma memories, mental defeat via the severity of negative appraisals about the self, and lack of self-referent processing via both appraisals and disjointed memories. Two additional models aimed to test the clinically relevant question whether the maintaining cognitive factors together with early symptom severity after trauma (Step 4) explain more variance in persistent symptom severity at 6 months than early symptoms at 2 weeks alone (Step 3).

We included trauma type and gender in the path models as survivors of interpersonal traumas (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso, Degenhardt, de Girolamo, Dinolova, Ferry, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Huang, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lepine, Levinson, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Scott, Stein, ten Have, Torres, Viana, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Koenen2017) and women (Olff et al., Reference Olff, Langeland, Draijer and Gersons2007) are at a higher risk of experiencing PTSD symptoms and would therefore be expected to also score higher on the cognitive factors.

Methods

Sample

Participants were recruited from the Emergency Department of King's College Hospital London following injury in a violent assault or road traffic collision (RTC). They were invited to participate in the study either by a research assistant when attending the Emergency Department or by letter, followed by a phone call. Inclusion criteria were: Aged between 18 and 65, trauma meets DSM-IV criteria for PTSD (American Psychiatric Association, 2000), able to read and write in English. Exclusion criteria were current psychosis or substance dependence, unable to remember the trauma, or assault in the context of ongoing domestic violence. These criteria were asked by structured questions at recruitment. Of 1291 people who were interested and suitable for the study, 828 provided data, and 700 participants provided PTSD symptom data 6 months after the traumatic event (see missing data analyses in the online Supplementary Material). Two smaller subsamples of the participants in this study also participated in studies of heart rate responses to pictures (Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Sündermann, Boellinghaus, Vossbeck-Elsebusch, Gamer, Briddon, Martin and Glucksman2010; Sündermann et al., Reference Sündermann, Ehlers, Böllinghaus, Gamer and Glucksman2010). There is no overlap in participants with other earlier studies conducted at the same emergency department (Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2007, Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013). Participants' demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1. All participants experienced a violent physical assault, and for 3 the assault also had a sexual element.

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and trauma types of the participants (N = 700)

Measurements

Cognitive processing

Peritraumatic cognitive processing was assessed using a short version of the Cognitive Processing Questionnaire (Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003). The short questionnaire contains a total of 22 items (α = 0.96) measuring data-driven processing (6 items, α = 0.91), lack of self-referent processing (5 items, α = 0.91), dissociation (i.e. derealisation and depersonalisation, 5 items, α = 0.88), and mental defeat during the traumatic event (6 items, α = 0.94).

Appraisals of the traumatic event and its sequelae

A 34-item version of the Post-traumatic Cognitions Inventory (Foa et al., Reference Foa, Ehlers, Clark, Tolin and Orsillo1999) assessed negative appraisals of the trauma and its aftermath, α = 0.95.

Trauma memories

The disjointed quality of participants' trauma memories was assessed with a short version (Sachschal et al., Reference Sachschal, Woodward, Wichelmann, Haag and Ehlers2019) of the Trauma Memory Questionnaire (Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003), which included 4 items, α = 0.80.

Safety behaviours

Excessive precautions were assessed with a 13-item version of the Safety Behaviour Questionnaire (Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers1999; Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001), α = 0.91.

Responses to intrusive memories

The Responses to Intrusions Questionnaire (Clohessy and Ehlers, Reference Clohessy and Ehlers1999; Murray et al., Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002) contains 19 items (α = 0.91) assessing thought suppression (6 items), rumination (8 items), and emotional numbing (5 items).

Persistent dissociation

The State Dissociation Questionnaire (Murray et al., Reference Murray, Ehlers and Mayou2002) comprises 9 items, α = 0.94.

PTSD symptom severity

The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale (PDS; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Cashman, Jaycox and Perry1997) assessed the severity of the 17 PTSD symptoms specified in DSM-IV-TR (American Psychiatric Association, 2000) at 2 weeks (α = 0.93). PTSD symptom severity at 6 months was assessed using the Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale, α = 0.95, and the PTSD Symptom Scale-Interview (PSSI; Foa et al., Reference Foa, Riggs, Dancu and Rothbaum1993; average ICC = 0.98); a composite score of both z-standardised measures was computed.

Further predictors

A trained specialist nurse coded injury severity from hospital records with the Injury Severity Score (Baker et al., Reference Baker, O'Neill, Haddon and Long1974). Trauma type was coded as 1 (assault) and 0 (RTC), gender as 1 (female) and 0 (male).

Procedure

The study was approved by the Institute of Psychiatry and South London and Maudsley Joint Ethics Committee (150/04) and the King's College Hospital Ethics Committee in London, UK (020/03). Participants gave written informed consent. Participants completed the Cognitive Processing Questionnaire at recruitment either in the Emergency Department within hours after their treatment for injuries or by post (which took up to 10 days after the traumatic incident). Appraisals, memory, safety behaviours, responses to intrusions, and persistent dissociation were assessed at 1 month via questionnaires. PTSD symptom severity was assessed at 2 weeks via questionnaire (PDS) and at 6 months by both structured interview with a trained psychologist (PSSI) and the PDS. Example items for all measures are provided in online Supplementary Table S1. Participants were reimbursed £60 for their time.

Statistical analyses

Path models (Wright, Reference Wright1934) were specified, in which all hypothesised effects and correlations were freely estimated. Unstandardized (b) and standardized (β) estimates can be interpreted as in multiple regression. We evaluated model fit using the χ2-test statistic (Jöreskog, Reference Jöreskog1969; Bollen, Reference Bollen1989) and the fit indices CFI (Bentler, Reference Bentler1990), RMSEA (Steiger and Lind, Reference Steiger and Lind1980), and SRMR (Jöreskog and Sörbom, Reference Jöreskog and Sörbom1981). As the variables had 4–7 response categories, they were treated as continuous for the use of a robust Maximum Likelihood methodology (see Rhemtulla et al., Reference Rhemtulla, Brosseau-Liard and Savalei2012). For the computation of prorated scale mean scores, at least half of the items had to be answered by the participant, otherwise the mean scores were regarded as missing data. Observed values deviated from univariate normality (see Table 2 for descriptive statistics) and were not multivariate normally distributed (Mardia's test p < 0.001). Therefore, we corrected the χ2-test statistic (method by Yuan and Bentler, Reference Yuan and Bentler1998, Reference Yuan and Bentler2000) and nonparametrically bootstrapped (Efron and Tibshirani, Reference Efron and Tibshirani1986) standard errors, which is also one method to determine significance of the indirect effects (Bollen and Stine, Reference Bollen and Stine1990). 95% adjusted bootstrap percentile confidence intervals for the indirect effects are reported (MacKinnon et al., Reference MacKinnon, Lockwood and Williams2004). We used the Full Information Maximum Likelihood algorithm (Arbuckle, Reference Arbuckle, Marcoulides and Schumacker1996), so that all participants with 6-month data could be included in the analysis (Enders and Bandalos, Reference Enders and Bandalos2001). The assumption of normality of the residuals of the models was found to be satisfactory. Results of a Monte Carlo power simulation (Muthén and Muthén, Reference Muthén and Muthén2009) indicated sufficient power to detect the hypothesised effects.

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the variables of the cognitive model and additional measures (N = 700)

Note. Mental defeat, lack of self-reference, peritraumatic dissociation, data-driven processing: 5-point Likert scale; Appraisals of the traumatic event/ its sequelae: 7-point Likert scale; memory characteristics: 5-point Likert scale; safety behaviours: 4-point Likert scale; responses to intrusions: 4-point Likert scale; persistent dissociation: 5-point Likert scale.

a For PTSD symptoms, the sum score is reported (4-point Likert scale); for all other measures, the mean of the scale is reported. Higher scores on all measures represent higher endorsements.

Data were analysed using RStudio (RStudioTeam, 2018) and the packages lavaan (Rosseel, Reference Rosseel2012), sjstats (Lüdecke, Reference Lüdecke2018), skimr (McNamara et al., Reference McNamara, Ariño de la Rubia, Zhu, Lowndes, Ellis, Waring, Quinn, McLeod, Wickham, Müller, Kirkpatrick, Brenstuhl, Schratz, Korpela, Thompson, McGehee and Kennedy2019), MVN (Korkmaz et al., Reference Korkmaz, Goksuluk and Zararsiz2016), and irr (Gamer et al., Reference Gamer, Lemon and Fellows Puspendra Singh2019). The R code can be retrieved from osf.io/k258j/.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Recruitment method neither influenced persistent PTSD symptom severity 6 months post-trauma, b = −0.05, β = −0.02, p = 0.41, nor did it moderate the prediction of persistent symptom severity by peritraumatic processing, b = 0.01, β = 0.003, p = 0.92. Assault survivors experienced more severe PTSD symptoms at 6 months than RTC survivors, F (1,696) = 38.10, p < 0.001, η p2 = 0.051. Women experienced more severe persistent symptoms than men, F (1,696) = 5.26, p = 0.020, η p2 = 0.007. Trauma type and gender interacted with each other, F (1,696) = 9.23, p = 0.002, η p2 = 0.013. The difference in persistent PTSD symptom severity between men and women was greater for assault than RTC survivors, p adj = 0.005 (see Table 1). Severity of injury did not predict symptom severity at 6 months, b = 0.01, β = 0.02, p = 0.55. 14 participants reported that they had received an evidence-based treatment due to the trauma, but this did not predict symptom severity at 6 months over and above early symptom severity, F (1,517) = 326.92, p < 0.001, R 2 = 0.39; ΔF (1,516) = 0.33, p = 0.57, ΔR 2 = 0.00.

Path analyses

Model fit of path analytic test of Ehlers and Clark's model of PTSD

The first step tested the core maintenance model (Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) which states that appraisals, disjointed memories and unhelpful strategies predict persistent PTSD symptom severity at 6 months post-trauma (see online Supplementary Fig. S1). Non-significant effects of trauma type and gender were set to zero. Modification indices suggested a correlation between safety behaviours and responses to intrusions, which was included. The resulting model fit the data, χ2(11) = 14.74, χ2/df = 1.34, p = 0.20, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.02 (0.00–0.05), SRMR = 0.02, and explained 52% of variance in PTSD symptom severity at 6 months.

The second step included the hypothesised effects of the four cognitive processing styles in the model. The non-significant effect of gender on lack of self-referent processing was set to zero. After requesting the modification indices, paths from peritraumatic dissociation to chronic dissociation, from mental defeat to disjointed memories, and from data-driven processing to PTSD symptoms at 6 months were included in the model. The resulting path model fit the data, χ2(30) = 37.96, χ2/df = 1.27, p = 0.15, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.02 (0.00–0.04), SRMR = 0.03 (see Fig. 2a), and explained 52% of variance in PTSD symptom severity at 6 months.

Fig. 2. (a) Path model for Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) cognitive model of PTSD. Cognitive processing during trauma was assessed within hours up to 10 days after the traumatic event; appraisals, memory characteristics, safety behaviours, responses to intrusions and ongoing dissociation were assessed 1 month after the traumatic event, and persistent posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms were assessed 6 months after the traumatic event. Trauma type is coded as 0 (road traffic collision) and 1 (assault), gender is coded as 0 (male) and 1 (female). Continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients, dashed lines with double headed arrows show correlations (in italics). R 2 = percentage of explained variance, ***/**/* = p < 0.001/<0.01/<0.05. (b) Path diagram of the cognitive model extended by early symptoms. Thick continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients for the predictions of the cognitive factors and early PTSD symptom severity, thin continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients for effects of trauma type (coded as 0 = road traffic collision and 1 = assault) and gender (coded as 0 = male and 1 = female), and dashed lines with double headed arrows and values in italics font show correlations. R 2 = percentage of explained variance, ***/**/* = p < 0.001/<0.01/<0.05.

Model parameters

All hypothesised path coefficients were significant, providing empirical support for Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model (see Fig. 2a). Table 3 summarises the direct, indirect, and total effects.

Table 3. Indirect, direct and total effects of variables derived from the cognitive model (N = 700)

Note. Indirect effects are calculated by multiplying the paths that lead from the variable to persistent PTSD symptoms at 6 months via specific mediators. Adjusted bootstrap percentile confidence intervals are reported. Confidence intervals not including zero indicate significant parameter estimates.

Direct and indirect effects of peritraumatic processing. Mental defeat, β = 0.17, p < 0.001, and lack of self-referent processing, β = 0.21, p < 0.001, indirectly predicted PTSD symptom severity at 6 months via both disjointed memories and negative appraisals, and via the maintaining strategies (see Fig. 2a and Table 3). Peritraumatic dissociation, β = 0.08, p < 0.001, and data-driven processing, β = 0.04, p = 0.016, indirectly predicted persistent symptom severity via disjointed memories and the three maintaining strategies (see Fig. 2a and Table 3). In addition to the hypothesized effects, there was a direct path from data-driven processing to persistent symptom severity, β = 0.08, p = 0.020, and a significant path from mental defeat to disjointed memories, β = 0.13, p = 0.019 (see Table 3).

Direct and indirect effects of appraisals. Negative appraisals at 1 month were predicted by the severity of mental defeat and lack of self-referent processing (see Fig. 2a). Degree of endorsement of negative appraisals at 1 month directly predicted PTSD severity at 6 months, β = 0.20, p = 0.001 (see Fig. 2a and Table 3). There were indirect effects via each of the three maintaining strategies: The total indirect effect of negative appraisals on PTSD symptoms at 6 months via safety behaviours, responses to intrusions, and persistent dissociation was β = 0.19, p < 0.001, and the total effect β = 0.39, p < 0.001 (see Table 3).

Direct and indirect effects of disjointed memories. Disjointed memories at 1 month were predicted by all four cognitive processing styles (see Fig. 2a). Severity of memory disjointedness at 1 month directly predicted PTSD severity at 6 months, β = 0.21, p < 0.001 (see Table 3). There were indirect effects via each of the three types of maintaining strategies: The total indirect effect of memory disjointedness (see Table 3) via safety behaviours, responses to intrusions, and persistent dissociation was β = 0.10, p < 0.001, and the total effect β = 0.31, p < 0.001 (see Table 3).

Effects of maintaining strategies. Each of the maintaining strategies at 1 month, safety behaviours, β = 0.09, p = 0.018, responses to intrusions, β = 0.11, p = 0.026, and persistent dissociation, β = 0.20, p = 0.001, predicted the severity of PTSD symptoms at 6 months (see Fig. 2a and Table 3).

Correlations. Mental defeat, lack of self-reference, peritraumatic dissociation, and data-driven processing correlated highly, and participants who endorsed negative appraisals to a greater extent also reported more disjointed trauma memories (see Fig. 2a). Safety behaviours were positively associated with responses to intrusions (see Fig. 2a).

Effects of type of trauma and gender. Having experienced an assault compared to an RTC was associated with more severe mental defeat, b = 0.78, β = 0.35, p < 0.001, lack of self-referent processing, b = 0.63, β = 0.26, p < 0.001, peritraumatic dissociation, b = 0.54, β = 0.22, p < 0.001, data-driven processing, b = 0.50, β = 0.21, p < 0.001, and more negative appraisals, b = 0.40, β = 0.18, p < 0.001 (see Fig. 2a). Women scored higher on mental defeat, b = 0.13, β = 0.06, p = 0.009, peritraumatic dissociation, b = 0.37, β = 0.15, p < 0.001, and data-driven processing, b = 0.35, β = 0.15, p < 0.001, than men (see Fig. 2a). Direct effects of trauma type or gender on PTSD symptoms at 6 months were non-significant, i.e. their effects on PTSD symptom severity were fully mediated by cognitive factors (see Fig. 2a and Table 3).

Role of early symptom severity in the prediction of persistent PTSD symptom severity

We fit a third and fourth path model to the data to investigate the relative role of early symptom severity and cognitive factors in the prediction of persistent symptom severity. The third model investigated the prediction of persistent PTSD symptom severity at 6 months by early symptom severity at 2 weeks, trauma type, and gender (see online Supplementary Fig. S2). The model fit the data, χ2(2) = 1.50, χ2/df = 0.75, p = 0.47, CFI = 1.00, RMSEA = 0.00 (0.00–0.07), SRMR = 0.01, and the three predictors explained 40% of variance in persistent PTSD symptom severity.

The fourth model included early PTSD symptoms in the path analysis of the cognitive model from Step 2 (see Fig. 2b). The non-significant direct path of data-driven processing and the non-significant effect of mental defeat on memory disjointedness were set to zero. The resulting model explained 53% of variance in persistent PTSD symptom severity, χ2(36) = 91.99, χ2/df = 2.56, p < 0.001, CFI = 0.99, RMSEA = 0.05 (0.04–0.06), SRMR = 0.03.

Early symptom severity and the cognitive factors explained more variance than early symptom severity alone (53% compared to 40%, z = 4.72, p < 0.001, Step 4 compared to Step 3). However, early symptom severity and the cognitive factors did not explain more variance in persistent symptom severity than the cognitive factors alone (53% compared to 52%, z = 0.37, p = 0.36, Step 4 compared to Step 2).

Discussion

This study was the first to comprehensively test the cognitive pathways to chronic PTSD symptoms specified in Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model of chronic PTSD in a prospective study of trauma survivors recruited in the first days after trauma. As predicted by the core model of the maintenance of PTSD symptoms (Step 1), negative appraisals of the traumatic event and/or its sequelae and disjointed memories at 1 month predicted symptom severity 6 months after the trauma directly and indirectly via maladaptive behavioural and cognitive coping strategies (of which safety behaviours, responses to intrusions, and persistent dissociation were tested here). Also in line with theoretical predictions, Step 2 showed that the degree of peritraumatic processing indirectly predicted PTSD symptom severity at 6 months via its effects on both, posttraumatic negative appraisals about the self and the world and the disjointedness of participants' trauma memories. In addition, data-driven processing during trauma also showed a small direct effect on PTSD symptoms at 6 months. Both path analytic models (Steps 1 and 2) explained 52% of the variance in PTSD symptom severity at 6 months after the trauma, which was a larger percentage than the percentage explained by early PTSD symptoms (40%, Step 3). Furthermore, the inclusion of early PTSD symptoms at 2 weeks post-trauma did not explain more variance in PTSD symptom severity at 6 months than the cognitive factors alone (53% for Step 4 v. 52% for Step 2, ns.). Thus, cognitive responses during trauma and in the first month afterwards strongly predict who will develop chronic PTSD, and have greater predictive power than symptom severity alone, which extends findings from previous prospective studies (e.g. Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Mayou and Bryant1998; Dunmore et al., Reference Dunmore, Clark and Ehlers2001; Halligan et al., Reference Halligan, Michael, Clark and Ehlers2003; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2007, Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a; Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008; Freeman et al., Reference Freeman, Thompson, Vorontsova, Dunn, Carter, Garety, Kuipers, Slater, Antley, Glucksman and Ehlers2013).

These results have practical implications for both the decision to whom treatment should be offered and treatment targets. First, NICE treatment guidelines (2018) currently recommend a period of active monitoring for trauma survivors with subthreshold symptoms. Thus, the severity of symptoms is used to determine prognosis. This study suggests that assessing appraisals, memory characteristics and unhelpful coping strategies could improve the accuracy of prognostic judgements. Second, the result that the effects of peritraumatic processing on chronic PTSD symptom severity was nearly fully mediated by the maintaining factors (appraisals, memory characteristics, and unhelpful coping strategies) supports the importance to distinguish between onset and maintenance factors of PTSD as suggested by Ehlers and Clark (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000), and the emphasis on maintenance factors in treatment. The maintaining factors investigated here are modifiable and can be targeted with psychological treatments. Cognitive therapy for PTSD, which directly builds on Ehlers and Clark's model, is one of the evidence-based treatments for PTSD (e.g. Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus, Fennell, Herbert and Mayou2003, Reference Ehlers, Clark, Hackmann, McManus and Fennell2005, Reference Ehlers, Hackmann, Grey, Wild, Liness, Albert, Deale, Stott and Clark2014). Furthermore, it has been shown that changes in appraisals drive symptom change in this treatment (Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Grey, Wild, Nussbeck, Stott, Hackmann, Clark and Ehlers2013) as well as in prolonged exposure (Zalta et al., Reference Zalta, Gillihan, Fisher, Mintz, McLean, Yehuda and Foa2014). Thus, changing overly negative appraisals about the meaning of the trauma for the individual's view of themselves and the world appears to be an important treatment goal that is addressed in evidence-based psychological treatments of PTSD (see also Schnyder et al., Reference Schnyder, Ehlers, Elbert, Foa, Gersons, Resick, Shapiro and Cloitre2015). Third, targeting the modifiable factors may offer the possibility for developing training programmes for the prevention of PTSD.

The results also are of theoretical interest. The inclusion of a measure of memory disjointedness demonstrated for the first time that although memory characteristics and appraisals were correlated, there are significant cognitive pathways to chronic PTSD that were mediated by memory disjointedness alone. The standardized total effect of memory disjointedness on chronic PTSD symptoms was β = 0.31. This is an important finding given that the nature of trauma memories has been the subject of considerable debate and supports theoretical models highlighting the role of perceptual processing and memories for trauma in PTSD (e.g. Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000; Brewin, Reference Brewin2014).

Negative appraisals of the self and the world at one month were important in the prediction of PTSD symptom severity at 6 months, with a total effect of β = 0.39. These findings are in line with other prospective studies (e.g. for a review see Brown et al., Reference Brown, Belli, Asnaani and Foa2019) and several theoretical models of PTSD that emphasize the role of appraisals in the development and maintenance of PTSD (e.g. Foa et al., Reference Foa, Riggs, Dancu and Rothbaum1993; Resick and Schnicke, Reference Resick and Schnicke1993; Ehlers and Clark, Reference Ehlers and Clark2000).

The results also showed that the well-established effects of trauma type (Kessler et al., Reference Kessler, Aguilar-Gaxiola, Alonso, Benjet, Bromet, Cardoso, Degenhardt, de Girolamo, Dinolova, Ferry, Florescu, Gureje, Haro, Huang, Karam, Kawakami, Lee, Lepine, Levinson, Navarro-Mateu, Pennell, Piazza, Posada-Villa, Scott, Stein, ten Have, Torres, Viana, Petukhova, Sampson, Zaslavsky and Koenen2017) and gender (Olff et al., Reference Olff, Langeland, Draijer and Gersons2007) on PTSD symptoms were fully mediated by cognitive factors. In line with previous findings that women experience higher levels of dissociation and greater loss of control than men during trauma (Olff et al., Reference Olff, Langeland, Draijer and Gersons2007), women reported more pronounced peritraumatic processing, which put them at greater risk of PTSD than men. Having experienced an assault was associated with greater peritraumatic processing and endorsement of negative appraisals compared to RTCs. Interpersonal trauma is more likely to affect negative processing about the self (e.g. mental defeat and self-blame), which is linked to higher rates of PTSD than accidental trauma.

The path analysis further allowed us to test how the four styles of cognitive processing specified in Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model contribute to chronic PTSD symptom severity. In line with predictions from this model, the effect of dissociation and data-driven processing on chronic PTSD symptoms was mediated by the disjointed nature of the trauma memories, whereas the effect of mental defeat was mediated by its effect on appraisals, and the effect of lack of self-referent processing via both disjointed memories and negative appraisals.

The study has several strengths but also limitations. It included a more comprehensive set of cognitive measures than previous tests of Ehlers and Clark's model and could therefore analyse nearly all causal paths specified in the model, with the exception of two further memory processes specified in the theoretical model, strong and generalised associative learning and perceptual priming, which were not assessed. The direct effect of data-driven processing on chronic PTSD symptoms observed in Step 2 is likely to be mediated by enhanced perceptual priming, which has been demonstrated in previous studies (e.g. Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Michael, Chen, Payne and Shan2006). These are thought to explain the easy triggering of reexperiencing symptoms by a very wide range of stimuli in PTSD and have been shown to predict intrusive memories and chronic PTSD (e.g. Ehlers et al., Reference Ehlers, Sündermann, Boellinghaus, Vossbeck-Elsebusch, Gamer, Briddon, Martin and Glucksman2010; Sündermann et al., Reference Sündermann, Ehlers, Böllinghaus, Gamer and Glucksman2010, Reference Sündermann, Hauschildt and Ehlers2013; Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehring and Ehlers2012b).

The decision to include trauma survivors with different trauma types increases the generalisability of the findings but also restricts the percentage of variance explained in chronic PTSD symptoms, which is likely to represent a lower bound estimate of the predictive power of the cognitive constructs (see Kleim et al., Reference Kleim, Ehlers and Glucksman2012a). Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model states that appraisals and unhelpful coping strategies used by an individual with PTSD are idiosyncratic and may vary by trauma type. The questionnaires used in this study could only assess common patterns, and thus represent incomplete assessments of an individual's appraisals and strategies. For example, we used a generic scale of safety behaviours and did not include a specific scale for safety behaviour around travel that is more predictive of PTSD in RTC survivors (Ehring et al., Reference Ehring, Ehlers and Glucksman2008). Even with the large sample size, we were not able to analyse separate path models for survivors by trauma type or gender, nor could we include these risk factors as moderators in the model. Finally, we also did not include early measures of negative appraisals and memory disjointedness, which would likely explain additional variance in early PTSD symptom severity.

In conclusion, the study supported the predictions from Ehlers and Clark's (Reference Ehlers and Clark2000) model of PTSD and suggests a focus on excessively negative appraisals, trauma memory characteristics and unhelpful coping strategies such as safety behaviours, rumination, thought suppression, emotional numbing, and ongoing dissociation in the early identification, treatment, and prevention of chronic PTSD.

Supplementary material

The supplementary material for this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0033291719002253.

Acknowledgements

We are very grateful to Emma Briddon, Melanie Walwyn Martin, Oliver Sündermann, Judith Kalthoff, Linda Horrell, Franziska Wallott, Marit Hauschildt, Anne Reitz, Sarah Auerbach, and Laura Pielmaier, for their help with recruitment, testing, and data entry. We thank the staff of the Emergency Department, King's College Hospital London for their support.

Financial support

The study was funded by Wellcome Trust grants 069777/200796 awarded to Anke Ehlers and David M Clark, and preparation of the manuscript was supported by the Oxford Health NIHR Biomedical Research Centre and a NIHR Senior Investigator Award. The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NHS, the NIHR or the Department of Health.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no conflicts of interest.

References

American Psychiatric Association (2000) Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: Fourth Edition, DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Arbuckle, LL (1996) Full information estimation in the presence of incomplete data. In Marcoulides, GA and Schumacker, RE (eds), Advanced Structural Equation Modeling. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum, pp. 243277.Google Scholar
Baker, SP, O'Neill, B, Haddon, W and Long, WB (1974) The injury severity score: a method for describing patients with multiple injuries and evaluating emergency care. The Journal of Trauma 14, 187196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benjet, C, Bromet, E, Karam, EG, Kessler, RC, McLaughlin, KA, Ruscio, AM, Shahly, V, Stein, DJ, Petukhova, M, Hill, E, Alonso, J, Atwoli, L, Bunting, B, Bruffaerts, R, Caldas-de-Almeida, JM, de Girolamo, G, Florescu, S, Gureje, O, Huang, Y, Lepine, JP, Kawakami, N, Kovess-Masfety, V, Medina-Mora, ME, Navarro-Mateu, F, Piazza, M, Posada-Villa, J, Scott, KM, Shalev, A, Slade, T, ten Have, M, Torres, Y, Viana, MC, Zarkov, Z and Koenen, KC (2016) The epidemiology of traumatic event exposure worldwide: results from the World Mental Health Survey Consortium. Psychological Medicine 46, 327343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bentler, PM (1990) Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychological Bulletin 107, 238246.10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bollen, KA (1989) Structural Equations with Latent Variables. New York: Wiley.10.1002/9781118619179CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bollen, KA and Stine, R (1990) Direct and indirect effects: classical and bootstrap estimates of variability. Sociologial Methodology 20, 115140.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewin, CR (2014) Episodic memory, perceptual memory, and their interaction: foundations for a theory of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 140, 6997.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, LA, Belli, GM, Asnaani, A and Foa, EB (2019) A review of the role of negative cognitions about oneself, others, and the world in the treatment of PTSD. Cognitive Therapy and Research 43, 143173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, RA and Guthrie, RM (2005) Maladaptive appraisals as a risk factor for posttraumatic stress: a study of trainee firefighters. Psychological Science 16, 749752.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clohessy, S and Ehlers, A (1999) PTSD symptoms, response to intrusive memories and coping in ambulance service workers. The British Journal of Clinical Psychology 38, 251265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunmore, E, Clark, DM and Ehlers, A (1999) Cognitive factors involved in the onset and maintenance of posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy 37, 809829.10.1016/S0005-7967(98)00181-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunmore, E, Clark, DM and Ehlers, A (2001) A prospective investigation of the role of cognitive factors in persistent posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) after physical or sexual assault. Behaviour Research and Therapy 39, 10631084.10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00088-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Efron, B and Tibshirani, R (1986) Bootstrap methods for standard errors, confidence intervals, and other measures of statistical accuracy. Statistical Science 1, 5475.10.1214/ss/1177013815CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A and Clark, DM (2000) A cognitive model of posttraumatic stress disorder. Behaviour Research and Therapy 38, 319345.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Mayou, RA and Bryant, B (1998) Psychological predictors of chronic PTSD after motor vehicle accidents. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 107, 508519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Clark, DM, Hackmann, A, McManus, F, Fennell, M, Herbert, C and Mayou, R (2003) A randomized controlled trial of cognitive therapy, a self-help booklet, and repeated assessments as early interventions for posttraumatic stress disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry 60, 10241032.10.1001/archpsyc.60.10.1024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Clark, DM, Hackmann, A, McManus, F and Fennell, M (2005) Cognitive therapy for PTSD: development and evaluation. Behaviour Research and Therapy 43, 413431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Michael, T, Chen, YP, Payne, E and Shan, S (2006) Enhanced perceptual priming for neutral stimuli in a traumatic context: a pathway to intrusive memories? Memory (Hove, England) 14, 316328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Sündermann, O, Boellinghaus, I, Vossbeck-Elsebusch, A, Gamer, M, Briddon, E, Martin, MW and Glucksman, E (2010) Heart rate responses to standardized trauma-related pictures in acute posttraumatic stress disorder. International Journal of Psychophysiology 78, 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehlers, A, Hackmann, A, Grey, N, Wild, J, Liness, S, Albert, I, Deale, A, Stott, R and Clark, DM (2014) A randomized controlled trial of 7-day intensive and standard weekly cognitive therapy for PTSD and emotion-focused supportive therapy. American Journal of Psychiatry 171, 294304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ehring, T, Ehlers, A and Glucksman, E (2008) Do cognitive models help in predicting the severity of posttraumatic stress disorder, phobia, and depression after motor vehicle accidents? A prospective longitudinal study. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 76, 219230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Enders, CK and Bandalos, D (2001) The relative performance of full information maximum likelihood estimation for missing data in structural equation modeling. Structural Equation Modeling 8, 430457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, EB, Riggs, D, Dancu, C and Rothbaum, B (1993) Reliability and validity of a brief instrument for assessing post-traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress 6, 459474.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, EB, Cashman, L, Jaycox, L and Perry, K (1997) The validation of a self-report measure of posttraumatic stress disorder: The Posttraumatic Diagnostic Scale. Psychological Assessment 9, 445451.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foa, EB, Ehlers, A, Clark, DM, Tolin, DF and Orsillo, SM (1999) The Posttraumatic Cognitions Inventory (PTCI): Development and validation. Psychological Assessment 11, 303314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freeman, D, Thompson, C, Vorontsova, N, Dunn, G, Carter, LA, Garety, P, Kuipers, E, Slater, M, Antley, A, Glucksman, E and Ehlers, A (2013) Paranoia and post-traumatic stress disorder in the months after physical assault: a longitudinal study examining shared and differential predictors. Psychological Medicine, 43, 26732684.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamer, M, Lemon, J and Fellows Puspendra Singh, I (2019) irr: Various Coefficients of Interrater Reliability and Agreement. R package version 0.84.1.Google Scholar
Halligan, SL, Michael, T, Clark, DM and Ehlers, A (2003) Posttraumatic stress disorder following assault: the role of cognitive processing, trauma memory and appraisals. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 71, 419431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jöreskog, KG (1969) A general approach to confirmatory maximum likelihood factor analysis. Psychometrika 34, 183202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jöreskog, KG and Sörbom, D (1981) Analysis of linear structural relationships by maximum likelihood and least squares methods (Report No. 81–88). Sweden: Department of Statistics, University of Uppsala.Google Scholar
Kessler, RC, Aguilar-Gaxiola, S, Alonso, J, Benjet, C, Bromet, EJ, Cardoso, G, Degenhardt, L, de Girolamo, G, Dinolova, RV, Ferry, F, Florescu, S, Gureje, O, Haro, JM, Huang, Y, Karam, EG, Kawakami, N, Lee, S, Lepine, J-P, Levinson, D, Navarro-Mateu, F, Pennell, B-E, Piazza, M, Posada-Villa, J, Scott, KM, Stein, DJ, ten Have, M, Torres, Y, Viana, MC, Petukhova, MV, Sampson, NA, Zaslavsky, AM and Koenen, KC (2017) Trauma and PTSD in the WHO World Mental Health Surveys. European Journal of Psychotraumatology 8(suppl. 5), 1353383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleim, B, Ehlers, A and Glucksman, E (2007) Early predictors of chronic post-traumatic stress disorder in assault survivors. Psychological Medicine 37, 14571467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleim, B, Ehlers, A and Glucksman, E (2012 a) Investigating cognitive pathways to psychopathology: Predicting depression and posttraumatic stress disorder from early responses after assault. Psychological Trauma: Theory, Research, Practice, and Policy 4, 527537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleim, B, Ehring, T and Ehlers, A (2012 b) Perceptual processing advantages for trauma-related visual cues in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Medicine 42, 173181.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kleim, B, Grey, N, Wild, J, Nussbeck, FW, Stott, R, Hackmann, A, Clark, DM and Ehlers, A (2013) Cognitive change predicts symptom reduction with cognitive therapy for posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 81, 383393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Korkmaz, S, Goksuluk, D and Zararsiz, G (2016) MVN: an R package for assessing multivariate normality. The R Journal 6, 151162. R package version 5.7.10.32614/RJ-2014-031CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lancaster, SL, Rodriguez, BF and Weston, R (2011) Path analytic examination of a cognitive model of PTSD. Behaviour Research and Therapy 49, 194201.10.1016/j.brat.2011.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lüdecke, D (2018) sjstats: Statistical functions for regression models. R package version 0.17.4.Google Scholar
MacKinnon, DP, Lockwood, CM and Williams, J (2004) Confidence limits for the indirect effect: distribution of the product and resampling methods. Multivariate Behavioral Research 39, 99128.10.1207/s15327906mbr3901_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marks, EH, Franklin, AR and Zoellner, LA (2018) Can't get it out of my mind: a systematic review of predictors of intrusive memories of distressing events. Psychological Bulletin 144, 584640.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNamara, A, Ariño de la Rubia, E, Zhu, H, Lowndes, J, Ellis, S, Waring, E, Quinn, M, McLeod, H, Wickham, H, Müller, K, RStudio Inc, Kirkpatrick, C, Brenstuhl, S, Schratz, P, lbusett, Korpela, M, Thompson, J, McGehee, H and Kennedy, P (2019) skimr: Compact and flexible summaries of data. R package version 1.0.5.Google Scholar
Morina, N, Wicherts, JM, Lobbrecht, J and Priebe, S (2014) Remission from post-traumatic stress disorder in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of long term outcome studies. Clinical Psychology Review 34, 249255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Murray, J, Ehlers, A and Mayou, RA (2002) Dissociation and posttraumatic stress disorder: two prospective studies of road traffic accident victims. British Journal of Psychiatry 180, 363368.10.1192/bjp.180.4.363CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muthén, LK and Muthén, BO (2009) How to use a Monte Carlo study to decide on sample size and determine power. Structural Equation Modeling 9, 599620.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
NICE (National Institute for Health and Care Excellence) (2018) Posttraumatic stress disorder. NICE guideline 116. Available at https://www.nice.org.uk/guidance/ng116/resources.Google Scholar
Olff, M, Langeland, W, Draijer, N and Gersons, BPR (2007) Gender differences in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychological Bulletin 133, 183204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ozer, E, Best, S, Lipsey, T and Weiss, D (2003) Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin 129, 5273.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Resick, PA and Schnicke, MK (1993) Cognitive Processing Therapy for Rape Victims. Newbury Park, California: Sage.Google Scholar
Rhemtulla, M, Brosseau-Liard, P and Savalei, V (2012) When can categorical variables be treated as continuous? A comparison of robust continuous and categorical SEM estimation methods under suboptimal conditions. Psychological Methods 17, 354373.10.1037/a0029315CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosseel, Y (2012) Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. Journal of Statistical Software 48, 136, R package version 0.6-3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
RStudio Team (2018) RStudio: Integrated Development for R. Boston, MA: RStudio, Inc. RStudio version 1.1.463.Google Scholar
Sachschal, J, Woodward, E, Wichelmann, JM, Haag, K and Ehlers, A (2019) Differential effects of poor recall and memory disjointedness on trauma symptoms. Clinical Psychological Science, 110.Google Scholar
Schnyder, U, Ehlers, A, Elbert, T, Foa, EB, Gersons, BPR, Resick, PA, Shapiro, F and Cloitre, M (2015) Psychotherapies for PTSD: What do they have in common? European Journal of Psychotraumatology 6, 28186.10.3402/ejpt.v6.28186CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steiger, JH and Lind, JC (1980) Statistically-based tests for the number of common factors. Psychometric Society Annual Spring Meeting. Iowa City, IA.Google Scholar
Sündermann, O, Ehlers, A, Böllinghaus, I, Gamer, M and Glucksman, E (2010) Early heartrate responses to standardized trauma-related pictures predict posttraumatic stress disorder – a prospective study. Psychosomatic Medicine 72, 301308.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sündermann, O, Hauschildt, M and Ehlers, A (2013) Perceptual processing during trauma, priming and the development of intrusive memories. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry 44, 213220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wright, S (1934) The method of path coefficients. The Annals of Mathematical Statistics 5, 161215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, K-H and Bentler, PM (1998) Normal theory based test statistics in structural equation modelling. The British Journal of Mathematical and Statistical Psychology 51, 289309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yuan, K-H and Bentler, PM (2000) Three likelihood-based methods for mean and covariance structure analysis. Sociological Methodology 30, 165200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zalta, AK, Gillihan, SJ, Fisher, AJ, Mintz, J, McLean, CP, Yehuda, R and Foa, EB (2014) Change in negative cognitions associated with PTSD predicts symptom reduction in prolonged exposure. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology 82, 171175.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Conceptual figure of cognitive factors leading to chronic PTSD according to Ehlers and Clark's (2000) model.

Figure 1

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and trauma types of the participants (N = 700)

Figure 2

Table 2. Descriptive statistics of the variables of the cognitive model and additional measures (N = 700)

Figure 3

Fig. 2. (a) Path model for Ehlers and Clark's (2000) cognitive model of PTSD. Cognitive processing during trauma was assessed within hours up to 10 days after the traumatic event; appraisals, memory characteristics, safety behaviours, responses to intrusions and ongoing dissociation were assessed 1 month after the traumatic event, and persistent posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms were assessed 6 months after the traumatic event. Trauma type is coded as 0 (road traffic collision) and 1 (assault), gender is coded as 0 (male) and 1 (female). Continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients, dashed lines with double headed arrows show correlations (in italics). R2 = percentage of explained variance, ***/**/* = p < 0.001/<0.01/<0.05. (b) Path diagram of the cognitive model extended by early symptoms. Thick continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients for the predictions of the cognitive factors and early PTSD symptom severity, thin continuous lines with arrows pointing in one direction show standardized path coefficients for effects of trauma type (coded as 0 = road traffic collision and 1 = assault) and gender (coded as 0 = male and 1 = female), and dashed lines with double headed arrows and values in italics font show correlations. R2 = percentage of explained variance, ***/**/* = p < 0.001/<0.01/<0.05.

Figure 4

Table 3. Indirect, direct and total effects of variables derived from the cognitive model (N = 700)

Supplementary material: File

Beierl et al. supplementary material

Beierl et al. supplementary material

Download Beierl et al. supplementary material(File)
File 10.6 MB