Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-vvkck Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-26T14:51:10.143Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Developmental models as frameworks for early intervention with children with Down syndrome

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 July 2011

Jacob A. Burack
Affiliation:
McGill University and Hôpital Rivières-des-Prairies
Katie Cohene
Affiliation:
McGill University
Heidi Flores
Affiliation:
McGill University
Jean-Adolphe Rondal
Affiliation:
Université de Liège, Belgium
Juan Perera
Affiliation:
Universitat de les Illes Balears, Palma de Mallorca
Donna Spiker
Affiliation:
Stanford Research Institute International
Get access

Summary

Our contribution to this book is the outline of a developmental approach that can be used to guide early intervention for individuals with Down syndrome (DS). For this task, we come fortified with frameworks, models, and paradigms from general developmental theory and research to guide our perspective on intervention. With this background and these resources, we suggest a universal context that is premised on the overwhelming similarities in underlying developmental processes that are observed among children, regardless of disability, and ability levels or individual experiences, as a first step to understanding intervention for children with DS from as early as birth and how it can be impacted by various direct and indirect factors (Hodapp & Burack, 1990). The emphasis on the commonalities of development does not obscure the obvious and important differences across etiologies, families, and individuals, but rather provides a framework of the whole child within which these differences can be discussed and understood (Zigler, 1967, 1969; Hodapp et al., 1990). Contemporary developmentalists celebrate differences at all levels of the human experience as essential, but not sole contributors, to developmental outcomes and behavior. Thus, the extensive understanding of universal trajectories of development and the ways that they are maintained or affected by cultural, societal, communal, familial, and individual differences is an organizing framework for understanding the relevance of the family, the individual, and outside influences to positive outcomes for children with DS (Hodapp, 1990; Hodapp & Burack, 2006).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bronfenbrenner, U. (1974). Developmental research, public policy, and the ecology of childhood. Child Development, 45, 1–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1977). Toward an experimental ecology of human development. American Psychologist, 32, 515–531.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The Ecology of Human Development: Experiments by Nature and Design. Cambridge: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1986). Ecology of the family as a context for human development: research perspectives. Developmental Psychology, 22, 723–742.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1989). Ecological systems theory. In Vasta, R. (ed.), Six Theories of Child Development: Revised Formulations and Current Issues, Vol. 6. Greenwich: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Burack, J. A. (1997). The study of atypical and typical populations in developmental psychopathology: the quest for a common science. In Luthar, S. S., Burack, J. A., Cicchetti, D., Weisz, J. R. (eds.), Developmental Psychopathology Perspectives on Adjustment, Risk and Disorder, pp. 139–165. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Burack, J. A., Hodapp, R. M., Zigler, E. (1988). Issues in the classification of mental retardation: differentiating among organic etiologies. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 29, 765–779.CrossRef
Burack, J. A., Hodapp, R. M., Zigler, E. (eds.) (1998). Handbook of Mental Retardation and Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Bybee, J. & Zigler, E. (1992). Is outer directedness employed in a harmful or beneficial manner by students with and without mental retardation? American Journal on Mental Retardation, 96, 512–521.Google ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. (1984). The emergence of developmental psychopathology. Child Development, 55, 1–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (eds.) (1990). Children with Down Syndrome: A Developmental Approach. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cicchetti, D. & Pogge-Hesse, P. (1982). Possible contributions of organically retarded persons to developmental theory. In Zigler, E. & Balla, D. (eds.), Mental Retardation: The Developmental-Difference Controversy, pp. 277–318. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Cicchetti, D. & Sroufe, L. A. (1976). The relationship between affective and cognitive development in Down's syndrome infants. Child Development, 47, 920–929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cicchetti, D. & Sroufe, L. A. (1978). An organizational view of affect: illustration from the study of Down's syndrome infants. In Lewis, M. & Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), The Development of Affect. New York: Plenum.Google Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. (1990). One road or many? Issues in the similar sequence hypothesis. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., Zigler, E. (eds.), Issues in the Developmental Approach to Mental Retardation, pp. 49–70. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. & Burack, J. A. (1990). What mental retardation teaches us about typical development: the examples of sequences, rates, and cross‑domain relations. Development and Psychopathology, 2, 213‑225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. & Burack, J. A. (2006). Developmental approaches to children with mental retardation: a second generation? In Cicchetti, D. & Cohen, D. J. (eds.), Developmental Psychopathology (Volume 3): Risk, Disorder, and Adaptation, pp. 235–267. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., Zigler, E. (1990). The developmental perspective in the field of mental retardation. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., Zigler, E. (eds.), Issues in the Developmental Approach to Mental Retardation, pp. 3–26. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., Zigler, E. (1998). Developmental approaches to mental retardation: a short introduction. In Burack, J. A., Hodapp, R. M., Zigler, E. (eds.), Handbook of Mental Retardation and Development, pp. 3–19. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. & Fidler, D. J. (1999). Special education and genetics: connections for the 21st century. Journal of Special Education, 33, 130–137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodapp, R. M. & Zigler, E. (1990). Applying the developmental perspective to individuals with Down syndrome. In Cicchetti, D. M. & Beeghly, (eds.), Children with Down Syndrome. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Inhelder, B. (1968). The Diagnosis of Reasoning in the Mentally Retarded. New York: Day. (Originally published, 1943.)Google Scholar
Mahoney, G., Fors, S., Wood, S. (1990). Maternal directive behavior revisited. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 94, 398–406.Google ScholarPubMed
Mans, L., Cicchetti, D., Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Mirror reactions of Down's syndrome infants and toddlers: cognitive underpinnings of self-recognition. Child Development, 49, 1247–1250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Merighi, J., Edison, M., Zigler, E. (1990). Motivational factors in mentally retarded functioning. In Hodapp, R. M., Burack, J. A., Zigler, E. (eds.), Issues in the Developmental Approach to Mental Retardation. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mundy, P. & Sheinkopf, S. (1998). Early communication skill acquisition and developmental disorders. In: Burack, J. A., Hodapp, R. M., Zigler, E.. (eds), Handbook of Mental Retardation and Development. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget's theory. In Mussen, P. H. & Kessen, W. (eds.), Handbook of Child Psychology: Vol. 1, History, Theory, and Methods. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wagner, S., Ganiban, J. M., Cicchetti, D. (1990). Attention, memory and perception in infants with Down syndrome: a review and commentary. In Cicchetti, D. & Beeghly, M. (eds.), Children with Down Syndrome: A Developmental Perspective, pp. 147–179. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R. & Zigler, E. (1979). Cognitive development in retarded and nonretarded persons: Piagetian tests of the similar sequence hypothesis. Psychological Bulletin, 90, 153–178.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H. (1948). Comparative Psychology of Mental Development (Rev. edn.). New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. (1957). The concept of development from a comparative and organismic point of view. In Harris, D. (ed.), The Concept of Development. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Werner, H. & Wapner, S. (1949). Sensory-tonic field theory of perception. Journal of Personality, 18, 88–107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigler, E. (1967). Familial mental retardation: a continuing dilemma. Science, 155, 292–298.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zigler, E. (1969). Developmental versus difference theories of mental retardation and the problem of motivation. American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 73, 536–556.Google ScholarPubMed
Zigler, E. (1971). The retarded child as a whole person. In Adams, H. E. & Boardman, W. K. (eds.), Advances in Experimental Clinical Psychology. New York: Pergamon.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. & Balla, D. (1982). Motivational and personality factors in the performance of the retarded. In Zigler, E. & Balla, D. (eds.), Mental Retardation: The Developmental-Difference Controversy, pp. 9–26. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Zigler, E. & Hodapp, R. (1986). Understanding Mental Retardation. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×