Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-42gr6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-24T08:42:02.939Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

14 - Cerebral constraints in reading and arithmetic: Education as a “neuronal recycling” process

from Part III - Brain, language, and mathematics

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Stanislas Dehaene
Affiliation:
Collège de France Paris
Antonio M. Battro
Affiliation:
National Academy of Education, Argentina
Kurt W. Fischer
Affiliation:
Harvard University, Massachusetts
Pierre J. Léna
Affiliation:
Université de Paris VII (Denis Diderot)
Get access

Summary

Overview

Cognitive neuroscience points the way beyond disputes pitting biological causes for behavior against cultural, experiential ones. Dehaene argues compellingly that the cultural tools of reading and arithmetic build directly on fundamental brain processes that are present in infants and other mammals. Common ideas in debates about learning and education seem old-fashioned and outmoded from this viewpoint – ideas such as that the mind/brain is a blank slate at birth, that there are innate, fixed mental organs, and that the brain is a learning machine capable of learning almost anything. The evidence is particularly clear regarding elementary numbers in arithmetic and the forms of letters in the alphabet. Specific, small cortical areas in the parietal lobe in primates and human infants are essential components for automatically detecting numerosity, even though has been no experience with the Arabic symbols for numbers. Indeed, there are even specific neurons tuned to different quantities from 1 to 5. Lesions in these areas produce acalculia (a number deficit). For reading, some restricted visual areas are dedicated to object recognition and to minute details of forms in space, invariant to size, position, or symmetry. These networks seem to form the foundation for building letter shapes, thus setting up the potential for children to learn the alphabet. Lesions in these areas produce alexia or dyslexia. For both mathematics and literacy, cultural objects (numbers and letters) make use of pre-existing brain architectures. In this way education can be understood as a ‘neuronal recycling process’ that builds on cortical structures.

The Editors

Type
Chapter
Information
The Educated Brain
Essays in Neuroeducation
, pp. 232 - 247
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allison, T., Puce, A., Spencer, D. D., and McCarthy, G. (1999). Electrophysiological studies of human face perception. I: Potentials generated in occipitotemporal cortex by face and non-face stimuli. Cerebral Cortex, 9(5), 415–430.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baylis, G. C. and Driver, J. (2001). Shape-coding in IT cells generalizes over contrast and mirror reversal, but not figure-ground reversal. Nat Neurosci, 4(9), 937–942.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butterworth, B. (1999). The Mathematical Brain. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Cohen, L. and Dehaene, S. (2004). Specialization within the ventral stream: The case for the Visual Word Form Area. NeuroImage, in press.CrossRef
Cohen, L., Lehericy, S., Chochon, F., Lemer, C., Rivaud, S., and Dehaene, S. (2002). Language-specific tuning of visual cortex? Functional properties of the Visual Word Form Area. Brain, 125(Pt 5), 1054–1069.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, L., Martinaud, O., Lemer, C., Lehéricy, S., Samson, Y., Obadia, M., et al. (2003). Visual word recognition in the left and right hemispheres: Anatomical and functional correlates of peripheral alexias. Cerebral Cortex, 13, 1313–1333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S. (1992). Varieties of numerical abilities. Cognition, 44, 1–42.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S.(1997). The Number Sense. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. and Changeux, J. P. (1993). Development of elementary numerical abilities: A neuronal model. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 390–407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., and Cohen, L. (1998). Abstract representations of numbers in the animal and human brain. Trends in Neuroscience, 21, 355–361.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Jobert, A., Naccache, L., Ciuciu, P., Poline, J. B., Le Bihan, D., et al. (2004). Letter binding and invariant recognition of masked words: Behavioral and neuroimaging evidence. Psychological Science, in press.CrossRef
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Cohen, L., Bihan, D. L., Mangin, J. F., Poline, J. B., et al. (2001). Cerebral mechanisms of word masking and unconscious repetition priming. Nat Neurosci, 4(7), 752–758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., and Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20, 487–506.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., Pinel, P., Stanescu, R., and Tsivkin, S. (1999). Sources of mathematical thinking: behavioral and brain-imaging evidence. Science, 284(5416), 970–974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Déjerine, J. (1892). Contribution à l'étude anatomo-pathologique et clinique des différentes variétés de cécité verbale. Mémoires de la Société de Biologie, 4, 61–90.Google Scholar
Eger, E., Sterzer, P., Russ, M. O., Giraud, A. L., and Kleinschmidt, A. (2003). A supramodal number representation in human intraparietal cortex. Neuron, 37(4), 719–725.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallistel, C. R. and Gelman, R. (1992). Preverbal and verbal counting and computation. Cognition, 44, 43–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hasson, U., Levy, I., Behrmann, M., Hendler, T., and Malach, R. (2002). Eccentricity bias as an organizing principle for human high-order object areas. Neuron, 34(3), 479–490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Isaacs, E. B., Edmonds, C. J., Lucas, A., and Gadian, D. G. (2001). Calculation difficulties in children of very low birthweight: A neural correlate. Brain, 124(Pt 9), 1701–1707.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lemer, C., Dehaene, S., Spelke, E., and Cohen, L. (2003). Approximate quantities and exact number words: Dissociable systems. Neuropsychologia, 41, 1942–1958.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lipton, J. and Spelke, E. (2003). Origins of number sense: Large number discrimination in human infants. Psychological Science, 14, 396–401.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Logothetis, N. K., Pauls, J., and Poggio, T. (1995). Shape representation in the inferior temporal cortex of monkeys. Curr. Biol., 5(5), 552–563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malach, R., Levy, I., and Hasson, U. (2002). The topography of high-order human object areas. Trends Cogn. Sci., 6(4), 176–184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McMonnies, C. W. (1992). Visuo-spatial discrimination and mirror image letter reversals in reading. J. Am Optom Assoc., 63(10), 698–704.Google ScholarPubMed
Miyashita, Y. (1988). Neuronal correlate of visual associative long-term memory in the primate temporal cortex. Nature, 335(6193), 817–820.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molko, N., Cachia, A., Riviere, D., Mangin, J. F., Bruandet, M., Bihan, D., et al. (2003). Functional and structural alterations of the intraparietal sulcus in a developmental dyscalculia of genetic origin. Neuron, 40(4), 847–858.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molko, N., Cohen, L., Mangin, J. F., Chochon, F., Lehéricy, S., Bihan, D., et al. (2002). Visualizing the neural bases of a disconnection syndrome with diffusion tensor imaging. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14, 629–636.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naccache, L. and Dehaene, S. (2001). The priming method: imaging unconscious repetition priming reveals an abstract representation of number in the parietal lobes. Cerebral Cortex, 11(10), 966–974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieder, A., Freedman, D. J., and Miller, E. K. (2002). Representation of the quantity of visual items in the primate prefrontal cortex. Science, 297(5587), 1708–1711.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieder, A. and Miller, E. K. (2003). Coding of cognitive magnitude. Compressed scaling of numerical information in the primate prefrontal cortex. Neuron, 37(1), 149–157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieder, A. and Miller, E. K. (2005). Neural correlates of numerical cognition in the neocortex of non-human primates. In Dehaene, S., Duhamel, J. R., Hauser, M. and Rizzolatti, G. (eds.), From Monkey Brain to Human Brain. Cambridge: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noble, J. (1968). Paradoxical interocular transfer of mirror-image discriminations in the optic chiasm sectioned monkey. Brain Res, 10(2), 127–151.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulesu, E., Demonet, J. F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., et al. (2001). Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291(5511), 2165–2167.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, S. (2002). The Blank Slate: The Modern Denial of Human Nature. London: Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Quartz, S. R. and Sejnowski, T. J. (1997). The neural basis of cognitive development: a constructivist manifesto. Behav. Brain Sci., 20(4), 537–556; discussion 556–596.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rollenhagen, J. E. and Olson, C. R. (2000). Mirror-image confusion in single neurons of the macaque inferotemporal cortex. Science, 287(5457), 1506–1508.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sawamura, H., Shima, K., and Tanji, J. (2002). Numerical representation for action in the parietal cortex of the monkey. Nature, 415(6874), 918–922.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shalev, R. S., Auerbach, J., Manor, O., and Gross-Tsur, V. (2000). Developmental dyscalculia: prevalence and prognosis. Eur. Child Adolesc. Psychiatry, 9(Suppl 2), II, 58–64.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanaka, K. (1996). Inferotemporal cortex and object vision. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 19, 109–139.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×