Project knowledge and its resituation in the design of research projects: Seymour Benzer's behavioral genetics, 1965-1974

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.shpsa.2018.04.001Get rights and content

Abstract

The article introduces a framework for analyzing the knowledge that researchers draw upon when designing a research project by distinguishing four types of “project knowledge”: goal knowledge, which concerns possible outcomes, and three forms of implementation knowledge that concern the realization of the project: 1) methodological knowledge that specifies possible experimental and non-experimental strategies to achieve the chosen goal; 2) representational knowledge that suggests ways to represent data, hypotheses, or outcomes; and 3) organizational knowledge that helps to build or navigate the material and social structures that enable a project. In the design of research projects such knowledge will be transferred from other successful projects and these processes will be analyzed in terms of modes of resituating knowledge. The account is developed by analyzing a case from the history of biology. In a reciprocal manner, it enables a better understanding of the historical episode in question: around 1970, several researchers who had made successful careers in the emerging field of molecular biology, working with bacterial model systems, attempted to create a molecular biology of the physiological processes in multicellular organisms. One of them was Seymour Benzer, who designed a research project addressing the physiological processes underlying behavior in Drosophila.

Introduction

In 1968, Seymour Benzer (1921-2007) provided the first proposition of a new research program on which he had embarked formally only one year earlier: the “search for defects in non-phototactic mutants describes the outline of a research program to attack the mechanisms underlying behavior by genetic methods” (Benzer, 1968, p. 52). A research program, or rather an actual project informed by a program, is a complex arrangement. As the quotation makes clear, it addresses a domain of phenomena under investigation (behavior), and typically a subset of the phenomena that can be studied in an exemplary manner (phototaxis), and it is based on an idea about the appropriate form of result (mechanistic explanation) and a suitable methodology (genetic methods). The goals (here, the mechanistic explanation) will be divided in several subtasks and the methods to achieve these tasks will involve several techniques specific to the material at hand that involve suitable instruments (e.g., behavioral screening devices), reagents (e.g., mutagens), and, in biology, typically an experimental organism (in this case, the fruit fly Drosophila melanogaster). Furthermore, employing the techniques, making the organisms available, and coordinating the work in a collective requires the project to be embedded in physical infrastructures, institutions, and social relations (in this case, a lab at the California Institute of Technology (Caltech), the social hierarchy of a principle investigator and the postdocs and graduate students working in the lab, and the Drosophila community and its systems of communication and exchange of protocols, mutant stocks, and other resources). In part 1 of this article, I will develop a framework for analyzing the kind of knowledge researchers draw on when designing and embedding a research project. The proposed account facilitates the identification of the sources of such “project knowledge” and the analysis of the processes of its resituation. The framework is developed from the case of Benzer's work in behavioral genetics presented in part 2, and in turn helps to understand how new research programs emerged around 1970 in biology and engendered conceptual change. The study thus also contributes to the history of this period, which was characterized not only by a molecularization of physiological processes, but also by the encounter of molecular biology with higher organisms, as well as new models of gene action (Burian & Thieffry, 2000; Morange, 1997; Suárez-Díaz & García-Deister, 2015). It furthermore contributes to the history of behavioral biology broadly construed.

Section snippets

Project knowledge in the design of research projects

“Research program” and “research project” are both actors' terms.1 Sometimes “research program” and “research

The researcher: Benzer's investigative pathway from bacteriophage to a metazoan model system

A central aspect of a research project is, of course, the individual researcher, or, more often, the group of researchers, who are part of communities and institutions, carry out the relevant activities, and communicate results to their peers. Most importantly, researchers possess or actively acquire the knowledge relevant to design their research project. Which knowledge they possess or which resources they are likely to draw from to acquire new knowledge strongly depends on their previous

Conclusion: Modes of resituating project knowledge in the attempt to create a molecular biology of multicellular organisms

Mary Morgan's (2014) account of resituating knowledge distinguishes between instances in which knowledge is transferred from one local setting to another, similar setting, and instances where a form of abstraction takes place that de-contextualizes a local finding, creating some causal or conceptual knowledge that can then be resituated in many, less comparable, localities. This matches with the notion, supported by the case study, that project knowledge can be domain-specific or generic (see

Acknowledgements

This article was written while I held a position at the University of Kassel and was revised when I was working in the Narrative Science Project (PI Mary S. Morgan) at LSE. Relevant material has been presented at the following workshops: Knowledge Transfer and Its Contexts, Center for Advanced Studies, LMU, Munich (September 2015); Working Across Species: Comparative Practices in Modern Medical, Biological and Behavioral Sciences, King's College London (January 2016); Many Methods – One

References (92)

  • M. Schindler et al.

    Harvesting project knowledge: A review of project learning methods and success factors

    International Journal of Project Management

    (2003)
  • C.K. Waters

    What was classical genetics?

    Studies in History and Philosophy of Science

    (2004)
  • A. Abrahamsen et al.

    Diagrams as tools for scientific reasoning

    Review of Philosophy and Psychology

    (2015)
  • R.A. Ankeny

    Fashioning descriptive models in Biology: Of worms and wiring diagrams

    Philosophy of Science

    (2000)
  • G.W. Beadle et al.

    Genetic control of developmental reactions

    The American Naturalist

    (1941)
  • W. Bechtel

    Generalization and discovery by assuming conserved mechanisms: Cross-species research on circadian oscillators

    Philosophy of Science

    (2009)
  • W. Bechtel et al.

    Discovering Complexity: Decomposition and localization as strategies in scientific research

    (2010)
  • S. Benzer

    Fine structure of a genetic region in bacteriophage

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1955)
  • S. Benzer

    Genes and behavior

    Engineering and Science

    (1968)
  • S. Benzer

    From the gene to behavior

    Engineering and Science

    (1972)
  • S. Benzer

    Genetic dissection of behavior

    Scientific American

    (1973)
  • S. Benzer

    Where behavior begins

    Engineering and Science

    (1974)
  • S. Benzer

    Interview by Heidi Aspaturian. Pasadena, California, September 11-February 1991

    (1991)
  • S. Brenner

    The genetics of Caenorhabditis elegans

    Genetics

    (1974)
  • S. Brenner

    Letter to Perutz [1963]

  • R.M. Burian et al.

    Introduction to the special issue ‘from embryology to developmental biology’

    History & Philosophy of the Life Sciences

    (2000)
  • R.W. Burkhardt

    Patterns of behavior: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and the founding of ethology

    (2005)
  • F.W. Carpenter

    The reactions of the pomace fly (Drosophila ampelophila Loew) to light, gravity, and mechanical stimulation

    The American Naturalist

    (1905)
  • C.F. Craver et al.

    In search of Mechanisms: Discoveries across the life sciences

    (2013)
  • D. Denno

    Human biology and criminal responsibility: Free will or free ride?

    University of Pennsylvania Law Review

    (1988)
  • G.S. Fraenkel et al.

    The orientation of Animals: Kineses, taxes and compass reactions

    (1961)
  • J.H. Fujimura

    Crafting Science: Standardized packages, boundary objects, and “translation”

  • S.C. Fujita et al.

    Monoclonal antibodies against the Drosophila nervous system

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1982)
  • C. Galperin

    From cell lineage to developmental genetics

    History and Philosophy of the Life Sciences

    (1998)
  • A. García-Bellido et al.

    Cell lineage of the imaginal discs in Drosophila gynandromorphs

    Journal of Experimental Zoology

    (1969)
  • D.C. Gooding

    Experiment and the making of Meaning: Human agency in scientific observation and experiment

    (1990)
  • R.J. Greenspan

    Seymour Benzer 1921-2007. Biographical memoirs of the national academy of sciences

    (2009)
  • J. Griesemer

    Tracking organic processes: Representations and research styles in classical embryology and genetics

  • K. Hentschel

    Visual cultures in science and technology: A comparative history

    (2014)
  • J. Hirsch et al.

    Sign of taxis as a property of the genotype

    Science

    (1961)
  • F.L. Holmes

    Investigative pathways

    (2004)
  • F.L. Holmes

    Reconceiving the Gene: Seymour Benzer's adventures in phage genetics

    (2006)
  • Y. Hotta et al.

    Abnormal electroretinograms in visual mutants of Drosophila

    Nature

    (1969)
  • Y. Hotta et al.

    Genetic dissection of the Drosophila nervous system by means of mosaics

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America

    (1970)
  • Y. Hotta et al.

    Mapping of behaviour in Drosophila mosaics

    Nature

    (1972)
  • View full text