Elsevier

Science of The Total Environment

Volume 520, 1 July 2015, Pages 114-119
Science of The Total Environment

Noise sensitivity and future risk of illness and mortality

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.scitotenv.2015.03.053Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Research is scarce on whether noise sensitivity is a risk factor for illness

  • Noise sensitivity did not show main effects on CVD morbidity or mortality

  • Noise sensitivity did predict angina pectoris in low employment grades

  • Noise sensitivity did predict the risk of future psychological distress

Abstract

Aircraft and road traffic noise exposure increase the risk of cardiovascular disease (CVD). Noise annoyance is the most frequent response to environmental noise. Noise annoyance has been shown to modify the association of transport noise exposure on CVD and noise sensitivity moderates the annoyance response to noise. This study uses prospective data from phases 1, 3, 5, 7 and 9 in 3630 male and female civil servants from the UK Whitehall II Study to examine whether a single question on noise sensitivity measured by annoyance responses to noise in general predicts physical and mental ill-health and mortality. Non-fatal myocardial infarction and stroke morbidity over the follow-up were defined by MONICA criteria based on study ECGs, hospital records, hospital admission statistics or General Practitioner confirmation. Depressive symptoms were measured by the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) and psychological distress by the General Health questionnaire (GHQ). There was no association between noise sensitivity and CVD morbidity or mortality except in people from lower employment grades where there was an association with angina. Noise sensitivity was a consistent predictor of depressive symptoms and psychological distress at phases 3, 5 and 7. High noise sensitivity scores at baseline predicted GHQ caseness at phase 3 adjusting for age, sex, employment grade, self-rated health and GHQ caseness at baseline (OR = 1.56 95% CI 1.29–1.88). Noise sensitivity has been identified as a predictor of mental ill-health. More longitudinal research is needed including measures of noise exposure.

Introduction

Recently there have been several studies linking prolonged aircraft noise exposure to increased risk of cardiovascular and stroke mortality (Huss et al., 2010, Hansell et al., 2013). These studies are part of accumulating evidence that both aircraft noise exposure and road traffic noise exposure are related to an increased risk of cardiovascular disease and mortality (Sorensen et al., 2011, Sorensen et al., 2012, Floud et al., 2013). The putative mechanism behind these associations is thought to relate to the stress hypothesis where prolonged noise exposure leads to increased stress responses, hypertension and increased risk of cardiovascular disease (Babisch, 2008, Jarup et al., 2008, Munzel et al., 2014).

The most frequent response to environmental noise is annoyance, which is a mixture of reported discomfort, anger and feelings of intrusion. Exposure response relationships have been found for road, rail and aircraft noise in which the degree of annoyance rises with increasing noise levels (Miedema and Vos, 1998). Annoyance has also been suggested as a possible moderating factor of the effects of noise on cardiovascular disease — as a subjective indicator of the degree of disturbance from noise that amplifies the stress response to sound (Babisch et al., 2013). However, noise annoyance levels are probably inadequate as a proxy for noise levels in associations with health outcomes. This is because there are non-acoustic factors, that may account for at least 35% of the variance in annoyance such as personality factors, attitudes to the noise source and perceptions of malfeasance related to the source of the noise (Job, 1988). Despite this, noise annoyance is associated with health outcomes, especially psychiatric disorder. In cross sectional studies it has been suggested that prior ill-health may lead to increased levels of annoyance and not the other way round (Tarnopolsky et al., 1980, Stansfeld et al., 1993). This has been explained as people who feel unwell being likely to be less tolerant of environmental discomfort.

Noise sensitivity, as a stable response to noise in general, is an independent predictor of the annoyance response to environmental noise (Job, 1999, Paunović et al., 2009, van Kamp et al., 2004). It has been postulated that noise sensitivity might be an indicator of vulnerability to environmental stressors, so that highly sensitive people might be more prone to develop illness when exposed to environmental noise (Stansfeld, 1992).

It is of interest to understand whether noise sensitivity does indicate vulnerability to ill-health, especially that attributable to noise, as this has implications for public health policy on reducing noise and advising noise sensitive individuals of the potential consequences of noise exposure. This is best attempted in longitudinal analyses. A single question on annoyance to noise in general was included in the first phase of the Whitehall II Study of British civil servants. We examined whether this question, which is an indicator of noise sensitivity (Job, 1999), is a predictor of future cardiovascular morbidity and mortality and psychiatric disorder. We hypothesised that with increased levels of noise sensitivity there would be a greater risk of both cardiovascular disease and psychiatric morbidity adjusting for ill-health at baseline.

Section snippets

Participants

The Whitehall II study was established between 1985 and 1988 with a target population of all male and female civil servants, aged between 35 and 55 years, in twenty London based civil service departments. 10,308 civil servants were examined in phase 1 of the study — 6895 men and 3413 women with a response rate of 73%, the true response rate was higher because around 4% of the invited employees had moved before the study and were not eligible for inclusion. The noise sensitivity question was only

Results

There were 3630 individuals in the sample, 49% were men. Overall, 48% of participants were sensitive, being highly bothered by noise in general. Noise sensitivity or being highly bothered by noise was more common in the 50–55 year age group (OR = 1.20 (95% CI 1.01–1.43)) relative to the 34–39 year age group. Women tend to be more sensitive relative to men (OR = 1.21 (95% CI 1.06–1.39)). Those in the lowest employment grade tend to be less sensitive than those in the highest employment grade (OR = 0.63

Discussion

Being highly sensitive to noise in general was more common in 50–55 year olds, women, and those of high employment grade, similar to findings in a national UK survey (Clark et al., 2014). As has been found previously in the literature, there were cross-sectional associations between high noise sensitivity and self-rated health and psychological distress (Tarnopolsky et al., 1980, Stansfeld et al., 1993). Being highly sensitive did not predict angina pectoris, non-fatal myocardial

Conclusions

Future research should be pursued in several directions. First, there is a need for a replication of these findings in a cohort study with noise exposure measures as well as noise sensitivity. In particular the associations between noise sensitivity and cardiovascular outcomes should be explored further in people of less advantaged social position. Less advantaged social position may be a marker for exposure to environmental stressors including noise exposure. If there is an association with

Acknowledgements

We thank all participating Civil Service departments and their welfare, personnel, and establishment officers; the Occupational Health and Safety Agency; the Council of Civil Service Unions; all participating civil servants in the Whitehall II study; all members of the Whitehall II study team. The first two phases of the Whitehall II study were supported by grants from the Medical Research Council; British Heart Foundation; Health and Safety Executive; Department of Health; National Heart Lung

References (35)

  • A.L. Hansell et al.

    Aircraft noise and cardiovascular disease near London Heathrow Airport

    Br. Med. J.

    (2013)
  • M. Heinonen-Guzejev et al.

    Genetic component of noise sensitivity

    Twin Res. Hum. Genet.

    (2005)
  • M. Heinonen-Guzejev et al.

    Noise sensitivity and multiple chemical sensitivity scales: properties in a population based epidemiological study

    Noise Health

    (2012)
  • M. Heinonen-Guzejev et al.

    Noise sensitivity and disability retirement: a longitudinal twin study

    J. Occup. Environ. Med.

    (2013)
  • E.M. Hill et al.

    Noise sensitivity and diminished health: testing moderators and mediators of the relationship

    Noise Health

    (2014)
  • A. Huss et al.

    Aircraft noise, air pollution, and mortality from myocardial infarction

    Epidemiology

    (2010)
  • L. Jarup et al.

    Hypertension and exposure to noise near airports: the HYENA study

    Environ. Health Perspect.

    (2008)
  • Cited by (60)

    • The role of aircraft noise annoyance and noise sensitivity in the association between aircraft noise levels and hypertension risk: Results of a pooled analysis from seven European countries

      2020, Environmental Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Therefore, the role of aircraft noise annoyance in the relationship between aircraft noise exposure and the risk of hypertension is still not clear. Noise sensitivity might be a general indicator of environmental sensitivity and have effects independent of noise levels (Stansfeld and Shipley, 2015). The direct association between noise sensitivity and noise-induced adverse effects has also been little studied, but conclusions are consistent: noise sensitivity was found to be associated with increased blood pressure (Otten et al., 1990), health complaints (including cardiac complaints) (Stansfeld and Shipley, 2015; Nivison and Endresen, 1993; Baliatsas et al., 2016), hypertension and the use of psychotropic drugs (sleeping pills, tranquilizers and pain relievers) (Heinonen-Guzejev et al., 2004; Okokon et al., 2018).

    • The role of depressive symptoms within the association of long-term exposure to indoor and outdoor traffic noise and cognitive function – Results from the Heinz Nixdorf Recall study

      2020, International Journal of Hygiene and Environmental Health
      Citation Excerpt :

      First, there was no possibility to test the individual noise sensitivity of participants. This factor was mentioned in some studies as an important modifier of the personal perception of noise (Stansfeld and Shipley, 2015), as people can strongly differ one from another in their noise sensitivity. Secondly, we excluded from the analysis participants who changed their residential address between the baseline and the first follow up as the information provided for the indoor noise was assigned to the baseline address.

    • Multisensory environmental sensitivity in patients with chronic tinnitus

      2020, Journal of Psychosomatic Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous studies indicated that increased sensory sensitivity to a specific modality may be part of a more general environmental hypersensitivity [15–17], and that this might be mediated by stress [18,19]. Further on, different types of environmental hypersensitivities have been linked in multiple studies to anxiety, depression, neuroticism and stress [12,20–23]. It therefore seems that chronic tinnitus and environmental hypersensitivity associate overlapping conditions as anxiety, depression and stress.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text