Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volumes 116–117, 27 May 2013, Pages 44-53
Physiology & Behavior

Different ontogenetic patterns of testosterone production reflect divergent male reproductive strategies in chimpanzees and bonobos

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.03.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Bonobos exhibit minimal increases in testosterone during puberty, unlike chimpanzees.

  • Increases in testosterone with age are clear among chimpanzee, but not bonobo, males.

  • Bonobos' stable testosterone levels are likely tied to reduced mating competition.

  • Testosterone production across development may be shaped by adult mating strategies.

Abstract

Male reproductive effort is often strongly related to levels of the steroid hormone testosterone. However, little research has examined whether levels of testosterone throughout development might be tied to individual or species differences in the reproductive strategies pursued by adult males. Here, we tested the hypothesis that inter-specific differences in male reproductive strategy are associated with differences in the pattern of testosterone production throughout early life and puberty. We compared testosterone levels from infancy to adulthood in two closely related species where levels of mating competition and male–male aggression differ significantly, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). We predicted that the reduction in male mating competition found in bonobos would be accompanied by a lesser developmental increase in testosterone production. We performed radioimmunoassay of salivary testosterone levels in a mixed-longitudinal sample of both species, collected from individuals living in semi free-ranging populations. This allowed us to examine the effects of development in a more naturalistic setting than possible in a zoo or laboratory. We found that among chimpanzees, testosterone levels declined slightly from infancy to juvenility, then remained low until increasing markedly during adolescence (with pubertal increases most pronounced among males). In contrast, there was little change in testosterone production with age in bonobos of either sex, with levels of testosterone consistent throughout infancy, juvenility, and the transition to adulthood. Our data are therefore consistent with the hypothesis that the ontogenetic pattern of testosterone production can be subject to rapid evolutionary change, shifting in association with species differences in male reproductive strategy.

Introduction

Investment in reproduction among males can be divided into both the production of gametes and the allocation of energy towards somatic and behavioral strategies that facilitate mating opportunities [1], [2]. The steroid hormone testosterone (abbreviated as T) is particularly important in influencing these latter two elements of male reproductive strategy, increasing muscle mass, enhancing libido, and stimulating aggressive and dominance behaviors in a given season or situation [3], [4], [5], [6].

While the association between testosterone and male reproductive effort has been well-documented in adults of numerous taxa, our understanding of how development mediates this relationship is less clear. According to life history theory, the production of testosterone across ontogeny should differ between species or individuals to facilitate the optimal allocation of energy toward growth, maintenance, and reproduction across the lifespan [1], [7]. Since high levels of testosterone can have a deleterious effect on the immune system [2], [8], production of testosterone may be minimized in situations or life stages where it is not sufficiently advantageous [5], [9]. Accordingly, testosterone levels typically remain low during juvenility, only beginning to increase at puberty in conjunction with reproductive maturation [10], [11], [12]. Despite this general developmental pattern being present across mammals, there may be important species differences in the precise patterns of testosterone production throughout development that reflect divergent male reproductive strategies in adulthood. This possibility is particularly compelling in light of the growing body of evidence that phenotypic changes between species commonly arise through evolutionary shifts in developmental trajectories [13], [14], [15].

Several studies have begun to investigate whether individual and species-level variation in the ontogeny of androgen production exist in association with differing adult reproductive strategies, using non-human primate models to examine these effects over an extended period of ontogeny. Within mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx) and chacma baboons (Papio hamadryas ursinus), individual differences in the production of testosterone during puberty have been found to correlate with dominance ranks among adult males [16], [17]. Similarly, in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus), males who retained subadult body size into adulthood (a viable strategy in this species to obtain sneaky mating opportunities without overt physical competition) were found to show smaller increases in testosterone during adolescence than males who developed their body size fully [18]. In addition, differences between baboon species in the timing and magnitude of the pubertal testosterone increase have been found to reflect inter-specific variation in the length of alpha male tenure and the association between rank and mating success [19]. These results support the hypothesis that within and across species, variation in the developmental trajectory of androgen production is central to the relationship between testosterone and reproductive effort among adult males.

These prior studies of testosterone production throughout development have largely focused on the pubertal increase in testosterone levels, since the period of adolescence represents an important transition between an individual's focus on growth and its focus on reproduction. However, individuals or species may also vary in their production of testosterone even before puberty. In a number of species, from humans to yellow baboons (Papio cynocephalus) and cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus), males and females show a neonatal elevation in testosterone that lasts for the first few weeks or even months after birth [11], [20], [21]. Though there is considerable debate about the function of this neonatal testosterone elevation [22], [23], one possibility is that variation in its duration or magnitude contributes to differences in reproductive capabilities among adult males [20], [24].

Here we test the hypothesis that species differences in male mating strategy are associated with variation in the ontogenetic patterns of testosterone production across the entire lifespan. We do so by comparing testosterone levels from infancy into adulthood in two closely-related ape species, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Chimpanzees and bonobos provide an ideal test case for this hypothesis, as they have been found to differ in both their reproductive strategy and in broader aspects of their ontogeny, despite having diverged from one another as recently as 850 kya [25].

Differences in male reproductive strategy between chimpanzees and bonobos appear to derive largely from the increased social gregariousness and sexual receptivity of bonobo females relative to chimpanzee females, presuming that the last common ancestor of the two species was chimpanzee-like [26], [27]. Bonobo females associate with males and engage in extensive non-conceptive sexual behavior throughout their menstrual cycle, while associations between male and female chimpanzees peak during the period of female sexual swelling, with copulations largely limited to this period [28], [29], [30], [31]. Correspondingly, it has been argued that competition for dominance rank and coercive aggression are less effective means of obtaining conceptive mating opportunities for bonobo males than for male chimpanzees [27], [32], [33], [34]. In support of this argument, bonobo males exhibit less frequent and less severe displays of aggressive behavior of all types than chimpanzee males, be it intra-group aggression, inter-group aggression, or even inter-specific predation [26], [35], [36], [37], [38], [39], [40]. Bonobos have also been found to show a lesser sex difference in androgen production relative to chimpanzees, lesser increases in male androgen levels when females are peri-ovulatory than found among male chimpanzees, and a weaker correlation on the whole between basal testosterone level and dominance rank among adult males [41], [42], [43], [44]. These two species therefore provide an excellent opportunity to test whether divergent male reproductive strategies in closely-related taxa are associated with broader differences in their endocrine maturation.

In addition to their divergent reproductive strategies, bonobos and chimpanzees have been found to differ in numerous aspects of their development. In particular, bonobos exhibit delays in development relative to chimpanzees in features of their morphology [45], [46], behavior [14], [15], [47], and cognition [14]. These distinctions provide support for the possibility that the ontogenetic pattern of testosterone production has also shifted between these two species, given the evidence from numerous taxa that hormones are a central mechanism in facilitating the pace of developmental transitions [48], [49].

No evidence exists at present to compare endocrine maturation between bonobos and chimpanzees, since to our knowledge no prior study of bonobo endocrine ontogeny has been performed. Existing studies of endocrine maturation in chimpanzees have consistently found that male testosterone levels increase during puberty, with these increases coinciding with growth in body weight and testicular volume [50], [51], [52], [53], [54], [55], [56], [57]. In captive populations, male chimpanzees begin to show elevations in testosterone between 6 and 7 years of age [50], [51], [52], [54], [57], with the onset of spermatogenesis occurring between 7 and 9 years of age [58], [59]. The only existing study of testosterone development among a small sample of wild chimpanzees indicates a similar developmental increase, occurring at a slightly later age [56]. Despite the relatively large number of studies documenting patterns of testosterone production throughout chimpanzee development, few have incorporated individuals from a broad developmental window (encompassing infancy, juvenility, adolescence, and adulthood). Moreover, these studies have primarily been conducted in laboratory environments, where asocial or minimally social housing conditions may have diminished any effects of dominance rank or social behavior on testosterone production [60]. This study represents one of the first opportunities to investigate testosterone production in chimpanzees ranging from infancy into adulthood, utilizing semi free-ranging study populations where individuals live in mixed-age and sex groups closely resembling those found in the wild [61].

Our major prediction was that bonobos would show a lesser developmental increase in testosterone production than chimpanzees, given their lesser mating competition as adults and their maintenance of numerous juvenile characteristics in adulthood [15], [27]. Our alternative hypothesis was that bonobos and chimpanzees would differ little in their ontogeny of testosterone production, given their genetic similarity [25]. We tested these predictions by measuring salivary testosterone levels from infancy into adulthood among bonobos and chimpanzees, making it possible for us to examine the contributions of both neonatal and pubertal testosterone elevations to the overall trajectory of testosterone production in each species. We examined testosterone in both sexes to assess the degree to which male patterns of development diverged from those of females.

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects for this research were chimpanzees living at the Tchimpounga Chimpanzee Sanctuary in Pointe Noire, Congo Republic and bonobos living at Lola ya Bonobo in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo. Both facilities house semi free-ranging ape populations living in mixed age and sex groups that have access to forest enclosures during the day and sleep in dormitories at night. Apes at these sites are provisioned but have access to natural food items in their primary forest enclosures.

Results

Though our analyses were performed with log-transformed testosterone (T) values, untransformed values are shown in Fig. 1 for illustrative purposes. For both chimpanzees and bonobos, the predicted models included the following terms: fixed effects of Age Category (4 levels, as described above), Sex (male/female), and Hour of Sample, and random effects of individual nested within year and a first-order ARMA term.

For chimpanzees (n = 971), analysis with the predicted model revealed significant

Discussion

Our results support the hypothesis that differences in male reproductive strategy between bonobos and chimpanzees are associated with important distinctions in the ontogeny of testosterone production between the two species. In chimpanzees, levels of testosterone increased in both males and females during the transition from juvenility to adulthood, doing so more markedly in males, in agreement with previous work [50], [51]. In bonobos, by contrast, there was no evidence of maturational

Acknowledgments

Thanks to Melissa Emery Thompson and Jean Maboto for their help in developing the saliva collection method, and to Alexandra Rosati and Rachel Carmody for advice in preparing the manuscript. Thanks to Steven Worthington for help in performing the statistical analyses. Thanks to Suzy Kwetuenda and the caretakers of Tchimpounga Chimpanzee Sanctuary and Lola ya Bonobo for their help in data collection. We are grateful to Rebeca Atencia, Lisa Pharoah, Debby Cox, Keith Brown, Claudine Andre, Valery

References (100)

  • M.S. Blank et al.

    Luteinizing-hormone sensitivity to naloxone in maturing male chimpanzees

    Brain Res Bull

    (1991)
  • M. Sobolewski et al.

    Territoriality, tolerance and testosterone in wild chimpanzees

    Anim Behav

    (2012)
  • J. Vittek et al.

    Direct radioimmunoassay (RIA) of salivary testosterone — correlation with free and total serum testosterone

    Life Sci

    (1985)
  • J.P. Higham et al.

    Measuring salivary analytes from free-ranging monkeys

    Physiol Behav

    (2010)
  • D. Granger et al.

    The “trouble” with salivary testosterone

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2004)
  • J. Dabbs et al.

    Male and female salivary testosterone concentrations before and after sexual activity

    Physiol Behav

    (1992)
  • R.G. Bribiescas

    Reproductive ecology and life history of the human male

  • M.P. Muehlenbein et al.

    Testosterone-mediated immune functions and male life histories

    Am J Hum Biol

    (2005)
  • P. Ellison

    Energetics and reproductive effort

    Am J Hum Biol

    (2003)
  • S. Bhasin et al.

    The effects of supraphysiologic doses of testosterone on muscle size and strength in normal men

    N Engl J Med

    (1996)
  • J. Wingfield et al.

    The “challenge hypothesis”: theoretical implications for patterns of testosterone secretion, mating systems, and breeding strategies

    Am Nat

    (1990)
  • G.R. Cunningham et al.

    Persistence of complete spermatogenesis in the presences of low intra-testicular concentrations of testotsterone

    Endocrinology

    (1979)
  • S. Stearns

    The evolution of life histories

    (1992)
  • M. Zuk et al.

    Endocrine–immune interactions, ornaments, and mate choice in red jungle fowl

    Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci

    (1995)
  • J.C. Wingfield et al.

    Avoiding the ‘costs’ of testosterone: ecological bases of hormone–behavior interactions

    Brain Behav Evol

    (2001)
  • M. Elmlinger et al.

    Reference intervals for testosterone, androstenedione and SHBG levels in healthy females and males from birth until old age

    Clin Lab

    (2005)
  • L. Gesquiere et al.

    Coming of age: steroid hormones of wild immature baboons (Papio cynocephalus)

    Am J Primatol

    (2005)
  • V. Wobber et al.

    Application of the heterochrony framework to the study of behavior and cognition

    Commun Integr Biol

    (2010)
  • J.M. Setchell et al.

    Developmental variables and dominance rank in adolescent male mandrills (Mandrillus sphinx)

    Am J Primatol

    (2002)
  • J. Beehner et al.

    Testosterone predicts future dominance rank and mating activity among male chacma baboons

    Behav Ecol Sociobiol

    (2006)
  • A. Maggioncalda et al.

    Reproductive hormone profiles in captive male orangutans: implications for understanding developmental arrest

    Am J Phys Anthropol

    (1999)
  • A.M. Andersson et al.

    Longitudinal reproductive hormone profiles in infants: peak of inhibin B levels in infant boys exceeds levels in adult men

    J Clin Endocrinol Metab

    (1998)
  • A.J. Ginther et al.

    Neonatal and pubertal development in males of a cooperatively breeding primate, the cotton-top tamarin (Saguinus oedipus oedipus)

    Biol Reprod

    (2002)
  • R.M. Sharpe et al.

    Role of the neonatal period of pituitary-testicular activity in germ cell proliferation and differentiation in the primate testis

    Hum Reprod

    (2003)
  • D.R. Mann et al.

    The neonatal period: a critical interval in male primate development

    J Endocrinol

    (1996)
  • D.R. Mann et al.

    Sexual maturation in male rhesus monkeys: importance of neonatal testosterone exposure and social rank

    J Endocrinol

    (1998)
  • Y. Won et al.

    Divergence population genetics of chimpanzees

    Mol Biol Evol

    (2005)
  • R. Wrangham et al.

    African apes as time machines

  • M.N. Mulavwa et al.

    Nest groups of wild bonobos at Wamba: selection of vegetation and tree species and relationships between nest group size and party size

    Am J Primatol

    (2010)
  • T. Furuichi

    Factors underlying party size differences between chimpanzees and bonobos: a review and hypotheses for future study

    Primates

    (2009)
  • K. Reichert et al.

    What females tell males about their reproductive status: are morphological and behavioural cues reliable signals of ovulation in bonobos (Pan paniscus)?

    Ethology

    (2002)
  • F. de Waal

    Tension regulation and nonreproductive functions of sex in captive bonobos (Pan paniscus)

    Natl Geogr Res Rep

    (1987)
  • R. Wrangham

    The cost of sexual attraction: is there a trade-off in female Pan between sex appeal and received coercion?

  • T. Furuichi

    Female contributions to the peaceful nature of bonobo society

    Evol Anthropol

    (2011)
  • M. Surbeck et al.

    Mothers matter! Maternal support, dominance status and mating success in male bonobos (Pan paniscus)

    Proc R Soc B Biol Sci

    (2011)
  • M. Muller

    Agonistic relations among Kanyawara chimpanzees

  • M. Muller et al.

    Male coercion and the costs of promiscuous mating for female chimpanzees

    Proc R Soc Lond B

    (2007)
  • T. Kano

    The last ape: pygmy chimpanzee behavior and ecology

    (1992)
  • T. Furuichi et al.

    Variation in male relationships in bonobos and chimpanzees

    Behaviour

    (1994)
  • H. Ihobe

    Non-antagonistic relations between wild bonobos and two species of guenons

    Primates

    (1997)
  • Cited by (27)

    • Non-invasive monitoring of physiological markers in primates

      2017, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      In humans, salivary steroid hormones have been measured, for example to determine adrenal activity and gonadal function (Lipson and Ellison, 1996; Riad-Fahmy et al., 1982). In captive non-human primates, steroid hormones, such as cortisol or testosterone have been measured in salivary samples of lemurs (von Engelhard et al., 2000), New World primates (Espinosa Gómez et al., 2015; Fuchs et al., 1997), Old World primates (Arslan et al., 1984; Boyce et al., 1995; Lutz et al., 2000; O'Brien et al., 2008; Pearson et al., 2008), and Great Apes (Behringer et al., 2014c, 2013, 2012a, 2009; Elder and Menzel, 2001; Heintz et al., 2011; Hohmann et al., 2009; Kuhar et al., 2005; Kutsukake et al., 2009; Wobber et al., 2013, 2010). However, saliva collection under field conditions is challenging.

    • Androgen metabolites impact CSF amines and axonal serotonin via MAO-A and -B in male macaques

      2015, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, these observations indicate that aromatase was constitutively expressed in macaque midbrain and that inhibition of activity with ATD did not significantly impact aromatase protein detection. Our data reconcile studies of elevated T, elevated aggression, and lower CSF 5HIAA in NHPs with studies that show low-activity MAO-A produced elevated serotonin and aggression in humans and rodents (Mehlman et al., 1994; Higley et al., 1996; Caspi et al., 2002; Huang et al., 2004; Newman et al., 2005; Alia-Klein et al., 2008; Wobber et al., 2013; Byrd and Manuck, 2014; Goetz et al., 2014). T or DHT increased TPH2 mRNA, which promotes increased serotonin synthesis; and the metabolism of T to E decreased MAO-A, decreased CSF 5HIAA, increased serotonin in axons of terminal fields and increased fenfluramine-stimulated serotonin/prolactin release (Bethea et al., 2013, 2014, 2015).

    • Human Mating Systems

      2015, Basics in Human Evolution
    • Puberty in Non-human Primates and Man

      2015, Knobil and Neill's Physiology of Reproduction: Two-Volume Set
    • Androgen receptor and monoamine oxidase polymorphism in wild bonobos

      2014, Meta Gene
      Citation Excerpt :

      Studies are needed focusing on the functions of the alleles in chimpanzees, bonobos and humans in order to understand the evolution of AR. Bonobos and chimpanzees are particularly adequate species to compare regarding AR polymorphisms, since it has been found that these two species were different in testosterone level change throughout ontogeny (Wobber et al., 2013). The weak points of this study are that our bonobo samples originated from two neighboring communities in the wild, and 13 individuals were mother–offspring pairs among the 32, while the chimpanzee and human samples that we compared our results with originated from several populations.

    • Age-related changes in urinary testosterone levels suggest differences in puberty onset and divergent life history strategies in bonobos and chimpanzees

      2014, Hormones and Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      However, due to small sample sizes of young individuals no clear overall developmental pattern for urinary testosterone metabolite levels could be discerned. Another research group reported that salivary testosterone levels did not change significantly with age in bonobos in either sex (Wobber et al., 2013). Compared to chimpanzees, infant bonobos had higher salivary testosterone levels and therefore showed a smaller increase with puberty (Wobber et al., 2013), which would suggest that unlike humans and chimpanzees, bonobos do not experience a juvenile pause.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text