Elsevier

Physiology & Behavior

Volume 94, Issue 3, 9 June 2008, Pages 491-500
Physiology & Behavior

Effects of isolation-rearing on the development of social behaviors in male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus)

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.03.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Social isolation has been shown to modify behavioral patterns and neuroendocrine function in many species. In this study, we examined the influence of post-weaning social isolation on the behavioral development and fecal testosterone and corticosterone levels of male Mongolian gerbils. In Experiment 1, isolation-reared (IR) males spent more time engaged in social-sniffing. IR males also showed more aggressive and anxiety-related behaviors than group-reared males (GR; three animals per cage). In Experiment 2, a screen-divided-reared condition (SDR) was examined in addition to GR (two males per cage) and IR males. In the SDR group, two subjects were separated by a wire mesh screen allowing for sensory communication and limited physical contact but not direct social interactions. SDR males showed similar patterns in sniffing and anxiety-related behaviors to IR males; however, an increase in aggression was not observed. No differences in fecal testosterone and corticosterone levels were found among males in different rearing conditions. These results suggest that post-weaning social isolation affects behavioral development without affecting testosterone and corticosterone levels. The behavioral changes observed may therefore be regulated by different mechanisms; increases in social-sniffing and anxiety-related behaviors may be caused by the deprivation of direct social interactions, whereas the enhancement of aggressive behaviors may be caused by the lack of sensory stimulation and limited physical contact with other individuals.

Introduction

Social deprivation in the early stages of life modifies adult behavioral patterns, neuroanatomy, and neurochemistry in many mammals. Animals reared in conditions of social deprivation show similar behavioral symptoms as humans suffering from psychiatric disorders. Research examining the effects of social deprivation using non-human primates and rodents are therefore a useful model for understanding the basic mechanisms underlying human psychopathologies (for reviews, see [12], [20], [46]). The effects of social deprivation vary according to the age at onset of isolation (e.g., neonatal, post-weaning, and adulthood), the length of isolation, and the type of isolation (e.g., from mother or peers) [12]. Post-weaning social isolation (i.e., isolation-rearing) is one of the most thoroughly investigated forms of social deprivation. In male rats, for example, isolation-rearing enhances locomotor activity in a novel environment [5], aggressiveness [48], anxiety levels [49], and endocrinological responses to stressful events [45]. Behavioral changes induced by isolation-rearing vary according to the species and specific strain [44]. For example, isolation-rearing induces greater changes in rats than in mice [6], [39]. These results suggest that the effects of social deprivation are influenced by the social structure of the species. i.e., gregarious species living in social units are more affected than less social ones. This notion is supported by results obtained using additional types of social deprivation. These studies have shown that highly social prairie voles are more susceptible to the effects of social isolation during the postnatal period or adulthood [31], [37] compared to their less social relatives (montane and meadow voles).

The Mongolian gerbil (Meriones unguiculatus) is a small desert rodent that originates from Mongolia and northern China [1]. In nature, this species forms colonies consisting of multiple males and females. Dominant males defend their territories by attacking intruder males or by scent-marking. Scent-marking is performed by rubbing the mid-ventral sebaceous glands on small objects in the environment [51]. Unlike mice and rats, reproduction within Mongolian gerbil colonies is limited to a particular pair [38], and both males and females are involved in raising the pups [4], [7]. These characteristics indicate that this gerbil is a highly social species. It is therefore conceivable that social isolation may have an impact on behavior in this species.

In fact, several studies have demonstrated that social isolation enhanced social investigation, aggression, and anxiety-related behavior in male gerbils [35], [36], [43], although available information is very limited as compared to mice and rats. For example, it is still unclear how gerbils behaviorally and endocrinologically mature during the period between the weaning and adulthood. Given that the gerbil is becoming increasingly popular as a model for anxiety and psychopharmacological research [3], [9], [32], further fundamental studies are needed to better understand the behavioral characteristics of this species. Moreover, most previous studies on the effects of isolation-rearing in mice, rats, and gerbils have examined behavioral changes that manifest in adulthood [13], [48]. That is, they have focused only on the result of isolation-rearing, not on the process. Very few studies have examined the effects of isolation-rearing from the point of view of behavioral development [21]. Therefore, as well as clarifying the ontogeny of social behavior (i.e., scent-marking and social interactions between individuals) and endocrine development (i.e., fecal testosterone and corticosterone levels) in male Mongolian gerbils, we investigated the influence of isolation-rearing on the behavioral development and endocrinological status during the post-weaning stage in Experiment 1 of this study.

In addition, Experiment 2 was conducted to investigate the environmental factors responsible for behavioral changes induced by isolation-rearing. Isolation-rearing deprives the subjects of sensory communication (e.g., olfactory, visually, auditory, and tactile stimulation) and direct social experience between individuals (e.g., play-fighting, allogrooming). Few studies have examined the effects of these factors separately [17]. To address this issue, we introduced a rearing condition wherein subjects were housed together in a cage but were separated from each other by a wire mesh screen. This setup allowed the animals to communicate using sensory cues but prevented them from interacting directly. By observing the behavioral and physiological changes of animals reared in this condition, along with isolated or group-reared animals, the effects of direct social interaction deprivation between individuals were investigated.

Section snippets

Animals

Twenty-two male Mongolian gerbils (MGS/Sea) were obtained at weaning (4 weeks old) from Kyudo Co. (Fukuoka, Japan). Upon arrival, the males were randomly assigned to two different rearing conditions: isolation-rearing (IR; one male per cage, N = 10) or group-housed rearing (GR; three males per cage, N = 12, each group consisted of individuals originating from different breeding colonies). Each experimental group was housed in an acrylic cage (440 W × 280 D × 180 H mm) with a wire mesh top. All animals

Scent-marking frequency

The frequency of scent-marking behavior increased from 6 to 12 weeks of age in both groups. IR males displayed a higher frequency of scent-marking than GR males (Fig. 1; F[1,20] = 12.6, P < 0.01).

Social interaction test

For all sniffing parameters (see Table 1), we found significant differences between males reared in IR and GR conditions. IR males spent more time sniffing nose and body regions (Fig. 2A & C; F[1,20] =69.7 and 6.11, respectively, P < 0.05), whereas GR males spent longer sniffing the anogenital area (Fig. 2B; F

Behavioral development of male Mongolian gerbils

By tracking the behavioral development of male gerbils from 4 to 12 weeks of age, behavioral changes from juvenile to adult patterns were clearly observed. For example, scent-marking behavior began at 6 weeks of age and gradually increased in a manner consistent with previous reports [27], [50]. Additionally, GR males in Experiment 1 and PR males in Experiment 2 spent more time engaged in anogenital-sniffing beginning at 8 weeks of age. Aggressive interactions in isolation-reared males also

Acknowledgment

This study was supported by Grants-in-aid for Scientific research from Japan Society for the Promotion of Science.

References (51)

  • C. Hunt et al.

    Faecal corticosterone concentrations indicate that separately housed male mice are not more stressed than group housed males

    Physiol Behav

    (2006)
  • M. Molina-Hernandez et al.

    Isolation rearing induced fear-like behavior without affecting learning abilities of Wistar rats

    Prog Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • M. Morgan et al.

    Incentive motivation and behavioral inhibition in socially-isolated rats

    Physiol Behav

    (1975)
  • E. Mostl et al.

    Hormones as indicators of stress

    Domest Anim Endocrinol

    (2002)
  • S.M. Pellis et al.

    Play fighting of rats in comparative perspective: a schema for neurobehavioral analyses

    Neurosci Biobehav Rev

    (1998)
  • B. Probst

    Individual marking activities not reflected by respective testosterone levels in male gerbils

    Physiol Behav

    (1985)
  • N. Sachser

    The effects of long-term isolation on physiology and behavior in male guinea pigs

    Physiol Behav

    (1986)
  • J.F. Sayegh et al.

    Effects of social isolation and the time of day on testosterone levels in plasma of C57BL/6By and BALB/cBy mice

    Steroids

    (1990)
  • M. Shimozuru et al.

    Social-defeat stress suppresses scent-marking and social-approach behaviors in male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus)

    Physiol Behav

    (2006)
  • M. Shimozuru et al.

    Scent-marking and sexual activity may reflect social hierarchy among group-living male Mongolian gerbils (Meriones unguiculatus)

    Physiol Behav

    (2006)
  • J. Spencer et al.

    Social isolation in the gerbil: its effect on exploratory or agonistic behavior and adrenocortical activity

    Physiol Behav

    (1973)
  • N.J. Starkey et al.

    The effects of individual housing on ‘anxious’ behaviour in male and female gerbils

    Physiol Behav

    (2007)
  • J.R. Stowe et al.

    Species differences in anxiety-related responses in male prairie and meadow voles: the effects of social isolation

    Physiol Behav

    (2005)
  • J.W. Turner

    Effects of sustained-release testosterone on marking behavior in the Mongolian gerbil

    Physiol Behav

    (1979)
  • E.I. Varlinskaya et al.

    Social behavior and social motivation in adolescent rats: role of housing conditions and partner's activity

    Physiol Behav

    (1999)
  • Cited by (26)

    • The anxiogenic effects of adolescent psychological stress in male and female mice

      2022, Behavioural Brain Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      These latter findings contradict previous studies reporting that stress in adulthood [68–70] and adolescence [71] results in decreased sociability. However, stress-induced increases in sociability have also been previously reported [72–74] and it has been proposed that mice may attempt to increase social interaction in order to buffer their anxiety in aversive situations [75]. We propose here that our findings of increased social preference and a female-specific decrease in investigation of a social odour in stressed mice is likely related to the pronounced heighted anxiety in these stressed mice.

    • Social isolation alters behavior, the gut-immune-brain axis, and neurochemical circuits in male and female prairie voles

      2020, Neurobiology of Stress
      Citation Excerpt :

      Interestingly, isolated prairie voles increased social sniffing toward unfamiliar conspecifics during the SA test. Social isolation can impair social recognition memory in other rodent species (Leser and Wagner, 2015; Liu et al., 2018; Shahar-Gold et al., 2013), increase social sniffing behavior (Shimozuru et al., 2008; Shoji and Mizoguchi, 2011), and alter olfaction independent of social memory (Gusmão et al., 2012). It would be interesting to further investigate our sniffing data by assessing whether social isolation impairs social recognition memory, social novelty/investigation, or both.

    • Social defeat and subsequent isolation housing affect behavior as well as cell proliferation and cell survival in the brains of male greater long-tailed hamsters

      2014, Neuroscience
      Citation Excerpt :

      The effects of social isolation may also be influenced by social structure. Non-social animals are usually less sensitive to isolation-associated stimuli in comparison to social animals (Shimozuru et al., 2008). In mammalian species, such as rats, mice, hamsters, voles, tree shrews, nonhuman primates, and even humans, adult neurogenesis (cell proliferation, survival, and differentiation of neurons) has been documented primarily in two brain regions: the dentate gyrus of the hippocampus (DG) and subventricular zone (SVZ) (Kempermann et al., 1997; Eriksson et al., 1998; Gould et al., 1998; Huang et al., 1998; Galea and McEwen, 1999; Tanapat et al., 1999; Bedard et al., 2002; Fowler et al., 2002; Curtis et al., 2003; Shingo et al., 2003).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text