Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 60, Issue 2, 2 April 2012, Pages 1250-1265
NeuroImage

Maturation of task-induced brain activation and long range functional connectivity in adolescence revealed by multivariate pattern classification

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.12.079Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study uses multivariate pattern classification analysis to examine maturation in task-induced brain activation and in functional connectivity during adolescence. The multivariate approach allowed accurate discrimination of adolescent boys of respectively 13, 17 and 21 years old based on brain activation during a gonogo task, whereas the univariate statistical analyses showed no or only very few, small age-related clusters. Developmental differences in task activation were spatially distributed throughout the brain, indicating differences in the responsiveness of a wide range of task-related and default mode regions. Moreover, these distributed age-distinctive patterns generalized from a simple gonogo task to a cognitively and motivationally very different gambling task, and vice versa. This suggests that functional brain maturation in adolescence is driven by common processes across cognitive tasks as opposed to task-specific processes. Although we confirmed previous reports of age-related differences in functional connectivity, particularly for long range connections (> 60 mm), these differences were not specific to brain regions that showed maturation of task-induced responsiveness. Together with the task-independency of brain activation maturation, this result suggests that brain connectivity changes in the course of adolescence affect brain functionality at a basic level. This basic change is manifest in a range of tasks, from the simplest gonogo task to a complex gambling task.

Highlights

► Multivariate pattern analysis discriminates fMRI maps of 13, 17 and 21 year-olds. ► Age-affected voxels are distributed throughout the brain. ► Age-distinctive activation patterns generalize across different cognitive tasks. ► Long range functional connectivity increases from 13 to 17 and 21 years of age. ► There is no direct relationship between maturation of connectivity and activation.

Introduction

Adolescence is a developmental period, between childhood and adulthood, of profound changes in body and behavior. These changes also include increased risk taking behavior, leading to rising numbers of accidents and early death, and an increased incidence of behavioral disorders in adolescence (Casey et al., 2008, Dahl, 2004, Steinberg, 2008). Although the neurobiological mechanisms underlying these changes are not yet well understood, it is likely that prolonged myelination and synaptic pruning play a role. Post-mortem studies have shown that these neural maturation processes continue well into young adulthood (Glantz et al., 2007, Huttenlocher and Dabholkar, 1997, Yakovlev and Lecours, 1967). Synaptic pruning is thought to underlie the decrease in gray matter density and cortical thickness observed in MRI studies, particularly in more complex association cortices (Giedd, 2004, Gogtay et al., 2004, Sowell et al., 2004). Myelination, on the other hand, is commonly associated with the linear increase in white matter volume seen in structural MRI studies (Giedd, 2004) and the protracted maturation of white matter bundles measured with diffusion weighted MRI (Lebel et al., 2008).

These anatomical changes are paralleled by wide-spread changes in functional brain organization. Brain-wide patterns of low-frequency temporal correlations in the fMRI signal during rest have shown a shift from childhood to adulthood in the strength of functional connectivity from short range toward long range connections (Fair et al., 2009, Kelly et al., 2009). Graph Theory analyses of whole brain networks have shown that network efficiency does not change with age, but that in childhood brain networks are organized more locally, based on anatomical proximity, whereas adult brain networks comprise regions spread over different brain lobes (Fair et al., 2009, Power et al., 2010, Vogel et al., 2010).

Many developmental fMRI studies have shown that task-related brain activity also undergoes changes during the transition from childhood to adulthood (for reviews, see Berl et al., 2006, Durston and Casey, 2006, Luna et al., 2010). The most consistent finding for cognitive tasks in the time window of adolescence is a maturation-related increase of BOLD response strength in the brain structures that are associated with task performance in adults (e.g. Crone et al., 2006, Keulers et al., 2011, Rubia et al., 2006). In other brain regions, hypothesized to be uncorrelated to task performance, the magnitude of brain activation tends to decrease or even disappear with age (e.g. Brown et al., 2005, Durston et al., 2006, Rubia et al., 2006). These age-related differences in response strength of particular brain areas during task execution are thought to be the consequence of brain-wide maturational changes in the way of interactions between neuron populations (e.g. Luna et al., 2010, Vogel et al., 2010).

A question fundamental to understanding functional brain maturation is then how the differences in task-induced activation relate to the changes in functional brain organization. It is well-established that there is a close correspondence between functional connectivity patterns, as evidenced from low-frequency BOLD fluctuations during rest as well as task performance, and the patterns of brain activity that emerge contingent upon task execution (e.g. Calhoun et al., 2008, Fox et al., 2006b, Smith et al., 2009). Specifically, the distributed functional organization of cortical areas that emerges during adolescent development largely overlaps with the clusters of commonly activated and deactivated brain areas that are consistently found during performance of a wide range of cognitive tasks (Cole and Schneider, 2007, Dosenbach et al., 2006, Dosenbach et al., 2007, Sridharan et al., 2008). Moreover, in adults there is a close relationship between the strength of functional connectivity between two brain sites during rest and their response strength modulation over trials during the execution of cognitive tasks (Mennes et al., 2010). Based on this close correspondence we hypothesized that particularly those regions that change their task-induced response strength with age will also show maturation of functional connectivity in the same age range.

This general relationship between changes in functional activation and in connectivity in the course of adolescent maturation may not hold for all task-induced activity, however. During adolescence, maturation is assumed to be associated with higher, more complex cognitive functions. Particularly well-studied are, for instance, response inhibition (e.g. Rubia et al., 2006, Rubia et al., 2007, Velanova et al., 2009), working memory ability (e.g. Crone et al., 2006, Geier et al., 2009) and risk taking behavior (e.g. Keulers et al., 2011, Steinberg et al., 2008, Van Leijenhorst et al., 2010), among others. For each of these abilities maturation may be associated with changes in specific brain structures involved in the ability and at a specific time window. In line with this, Cohen et al. (2010) were able to predict age from functional activation maps depicting the activation differences between successful stop trials and go trials, but not from the activation associated with go trials compared to rest. This suggests that maturation related differences in brain activity may be function specific.

In the present study, we investigated the patterns of maturation in both functional connectivity estimated from low-frequency BOLD signal fluctuations and functional activation related to task execution. Brain-wide maturation patterns will be quantified with multivariate pattern classification (MVP) tools (for reviews, see Haynes and Rees, 2006, Pereira et al., 2009). These tools use machine learning principles to find the pattern in a multidimensional space (e.g. the voxels of the images) that distinguishes two classes of examples. MVP analysis was introduced in the neuroimaging literature to “read” the voxel response patterns in visual cortex associated with particular stimulus categories (e.g. Cox and Savoy, 2003, Haxby et al., 2001), but has since then also been used across subjects, for instance, to find the distributed pattern that characterizes neuropathology (e.g. Fan et al., 2007, Sun et al., 2009). Here we will use MVP to classify functional brain maps according to age. Sensitivity of MVP to developmental patterns has recently been demonstrated (Cohen et al., 2010, Dosenbach et al., 2010). MVP analysis is generally more sensitive than the traditional univariate general linear model (GLM) analysis (e.g. De Martino et al., 2008). Moreover, MVP allows to directly test the generalizability of a distinctive pattern learned from a particular set of examples to a new set of example data, to establish whether they carry the same age-discriminative information. This feature allows us to directly test the functional specificity of maturation patterns by training a classifier on functional maps derived from a particular task and testing on data acquired during another task.

First of all, we established the sensitivity of the MVP classification to characterize maturation processes that take place during adolescence. We made use of brain activation data during the performance of a gonogo task, obtained from participants in three age groups, respectively 13, 17 and 21 years of age. The gonogo task focuses on neural processes related to the inhibition of a preponent response and has been frequently used in neurodevelopmental studies (e.g. Durston et al., 2006, Rubia et al., 2006, Tamm et al., 2002). In the task used here, participants had to press a response key whenever a letter was presented, but had to withhold their response when the infrequent nogo stimulus was presented (letter “A”). Next to the MVP classification analysis, we applied the traditional univariate voxel-wise analysis to establish age-related functional differences between the different age groups.

Secondly, we studied the functional specificity of the maturation patterns found in the task-related brain activity. The intrinsic generalization step of MVP classification allowed us to address this question in a direct way. Different cognitive tasks intend to steer specific neurocognitive processes. By using data from one particular task for training and data from a completely different task for testing, it can be directly investigated whether the pattern learned is associated with a specific cognitive function unique to the training task. Therefore, we also used fMRI data obtained from the same sample of participants while they performed a challenging gambling task (Keulers et al., 2011) and investigated whether the classifier trained on the data from the simple gonogo task was successful in classifying participants according to age from their functional activation maps during the complex gambling task, and vice versa.

Thirdly, we studied the relationship of task-related maturation patterns to the differences in short and long range low-frequency functional connectivities that have been described from childhood to early adulthood (Fair et al., 2007a, Fair et al., 2008, Fair et al., 2009, Kelly et al., 2009). We determined the strength of connectedness (Bullmore and Sporns, 2009) of gray matter voxels based on low frequency signal fluctuations in the gonogo data. Although low-frequency data obtained during task performance yield small differences in the distribution of functional connectivity compared to data obtained during rest, the overall lay-out of networks between the data is very similar (Dosenbach et al., 2010, Fair et al., 2007b). We investigated whether voxels that contributed to task-related age classification in the MVP differed from other voxels in their functional connectedness with the rest of the brain. In addition, in a whole brain exploratory analysis we entered the short and long range connectivity strength maps into a MVP classification analysis to allow a visualization of the maturation in functional connections and to compare these with maturational patterns in brain activity during task execution.

Section snippets

Participants and procedure

A total of 70 participants from the seventh (12/13 year-olds) and eleventh (16/17 year-olds) grades of pre-university education as well as university students (20/21 year-olds) were included. This sample was also described in a previous study, which examined developmental differences in brain activation during a challenging gambling task (Keulers et al., 2011). Participants were recruited through advertisements and information talks. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, were

Behavioral performance

The behavioral results showed an overall decrease in mean reaction time with age (F(2, 54) = 13.13; p < .001). The 13 year-olds reacted slower compared with 17 (p = .022) and 21 (<.001) year-olds, and 17 year-olds in their part were slower than 21 year-olds (p = .043). In addition, the ANOVA showed a main effect of trial type with longer reaction times on Go oddball compared with Go trials (F(2, 54) = 14.25; p < .001). This suggests that reacting on less frequent Go oddball trials is more difficult compared

Discussion

Multivariate pattern (MVP) analysis allowed a reliable classification of adolescent age groups of respectively 13, 17 and 21 year olds based on their activation pattern during a simple gonogo task. The MVP results showed that developmental differences in task activation are spatially distributed throughout the brain, affecting the responsiveness of a wide range of cortical and subcortical structures. We showed that these distributed age-distinctive patterns generalize from one cognitive task to

References (93)

  • J. Duncan et al.

    Common regions of the human frontal lobe recruited by diverse cognitive demands

    TINS

    (2000)
  • S. Durston et al.

    What have we learned about cognitive development from neuroimaging?

    Neuropsychologia

    (2006)
  • D.A. Fair et al.

    A method for using blocked and event-related fMRI data to study “resting state” functional connectivity

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • Y. Fan et al.

    Multivariate examination of brain abnormality using both structural and functional MRI

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • C.R. Genovese et al.

    Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • A. Giorgio et al.

    Longitudinal changes in grey and white matter during adolescence

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • L.A. Glantz et al.

    Synaptophysin and postsynaptic density protein 95 in the human prefrontal cortex from mid-gestation into early adulthood

    Neuroscience

    (2007)
  • A. Hampshire et al.

    The role of the right inferior frontal gyrus: inhibition and attentional control

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • E.H.H. Keulers et al.

    Developmental changes between ages 13 and 21 years in the extent and magnitude of the BOLD response during decision making

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • C. Lebel et al.

    Microstructural maturation of the human brain from childhood to adulthood

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • M. Mennes et al.

    Inter-individual differences in resting state functional connectivity predict task-induced BOLD activity

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • J. Mourao-Miranda et al.

    Classifying brain states and determining the discriminating activation patterns: Support Vector Machine on functional MRI data

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • F. Pereira et al.

    Machine learning classifiers and fMRI: a tutorial overview

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • J.D. Power et al.

    The development of human functional brain networks

    Neuron

    (2010)
  • S. Ryali et al.

    Sparse logistic regression for whole brain classification of fMRI data

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • N. Staeren et al.

    Sound categories are represented as distributed patterns in the human auditory cortex

    Curr. Biol.

    (2009)
  • L. Steinberg

    A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking

    Dev. Rev.

    (2008)
  • P. Stiers et al.

    Distributed task coding throughout the multiple demand network of the human frontal-insular cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • D. Sun et al.

    Elucidating a magnetic resonance imaging-based neuroanatomic biomarker for psychosis: classification analysis using probabilistic brain atlas and machine learning algorithms

    Biol. Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • S. Yoo et al.

    Head motion analysis during cognitive fMRI examination: application in patients with schizophrenia

    Neurosci. Res.

    (2005)
  • T.M. Achenbach et al.

    Manual for the ASEBA School-age Forms & Profiles

    (2001)
  • T.M. Achenbach et al.

    Manual for ASEBA Adult Forms and Profiles

    (2003)
  • A. Ardila et al.

    The influence of the parents' educational level on the development of executive functions

    Dev. Neuropsychol.

    (2005)
  • C.F. Beckmann et al.

    Investigations into resting state connectivity using independent component analysis

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B

    (2005)
  • T.T. Brown et al.

    Developmental changes in human cerebral functional organization for word generation

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2005)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • E. Bullmore et al.

    Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2009)
  • V.D. Calhoun et al.

    Modulation of temporally coherent brain networks estimated using ICA at rest and during cognitive tasks

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (2008)
  • J. Chikazoe et al.

    Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2009)
  • J.R. Cohen et al.

    Decoding developmental differences and individual variability in response inhibition through predictive analyses across individuals

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • M. Corbetta et al.

    Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • E.A. Crone et al.

    Neurocognitive development of the ability to manipulate information in working memory

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2006)
  • R.E. Dahl

    Adolescent brain development: a period of vulnerabilities and opportunities. Keynote address

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2004)
  • S.W. Dale et al.

    Correlated low-frequency BOLD fluctuations in the resting human brain are modulated by recent experience in category-preferential visual regions

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2010)
  • J.S. Damoiseaux et al.

    Consistent resting-state networks across healthy subjects

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2006)
  • Directoraat-Generaal voor de Arbeidsvoorziening

    Handleiding voor de functieanalyse [Function analyses manual]

    (1989)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text