Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 46, Issue 3, 1 July 2009, Pages 844-853
NeuroImage

Visual features of an observed agent do not modulate human brain activity during action observation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.03.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Recent neuroimaging evidence in macaques has shown that the neural system underlying the observation of hand actions performed by others (i.e., “action observation system”) is modulated by whether the observed action is performed by a person in full view or an isolated hand (i.e., type of view manipulation). Although a human homologue of such circuit has been identified, whether in humans the neural processes involved in this capacity are modulated by the type of view remains unknown. Here we used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to investigate whether the “action observation system”, with specific reference to the ventral premotor cortex, responds differentially depending on type of view. We also tested this manipulation within regions of the human brain showing overlapping activity for both the observation and the execution of action (“mirror” regions). To this end, the same subjects were requested to observe grasping actions performed under the two types of view (observation conditions) or to perform a grasping action (execution condition). Results from whole-brain analyses indicate that overlapping activity for action observation and execution was evident in a broad network of areas including parietal, premotor and temporal cortices. Activity within such network was evident for both the observation of a person in full view or an isolated hand, but it was not modulated by the type of view. Similarly, results from region of interest (ROI) analyses, performed within the ventral premotor cortex, did confirm that this area responded in a similar fashion following the observation of either an isolated hand or an entire model acting. These findings offer novel insights on what the “action observation” and the “mirror” systems visually code and how the processing underlying such coding may vary across species. Further, they support the hypothesis that action goal is amongst the main determinants for the revelation of action observation activity, and to the existence of a broad system involved in the simulation of action.

Introduction

Mirror neurons are a class of visuomotor neurons activated by both the execution and the passive observation of object-related actions. Cells having this property were found in macaques within the convexity behind the arcuate sulcus (area F5c) within the premotor cortex (Di Pellegrino et al., 1992, Gallese et al., 1996, Rizzolatti et al., 1996a), and in the complex PF/PFG (PF) within the rostral part of the convexity of the inferior parietal cortex (Gallese et al., 2002, Fogassi et al., 2005).

Following this discovery, many functional resonance imaging (fMRI) studies have been performed in order to uncover a similar system in humans (for review see Dinstein et al., 2008, Turella et al., 2009). Amongst these studies the most convincing evidence of a “mirror-like” system in humans comes from a study by Gazzola et al., 2007a, Gazzola et al., 2007b and Gazzola and Keysers (2008) who tested both action execution and observation within the same individuals. In one day they asked participants to observe either a human model or an industrial robot performing a variety of actions and in a separate day to perform the actions. They found regions of overlap for action observation and execution in classic ‘mirror’ areas together with many areas which were not previously considered as mirror (Gazzola et al., 2007a, Gazzola et al., 2007b, Gazzola and Keysers, 2008). Further, another interesting finding stemming from this work is that the mirror system is similarly activated by the sight of both the human and the robotic hand. This occurred despite the movement of the two agents exhibiting dramatically different kinematics. This was taken as the evidence that action goal rather than a tight kinematic match is amongst the main determinants for the revelation of mirror activity.

In this connection, recent findings from a fMRI study investigating the neural underpinnings of action observation in monkeys add a further level of complexity regarding the visual requirements necessary to activate the ‘observation’ component of the mirror system (Nelissen et al., 2005). Monkeys observed video clips showing a full view of a person grasping an object or an isolated hand grasping objects and static single frames or scrambled videos as controls. It was found that premotor area F5c (the area in which mirror neurons were first discovered) was active only when the monkey observed a human model presented in her entirety grasping an object, but it was not active when it observed a human hand detached from the body performing the task. In the other subregions of F5 (i.e., F5a, F5p) activation due to action observation was reported for both the model and the hand alone acting. These results seem to suggest that the type of view alerts different sectors of the premotor cortex and this occurs despite the fact that the goal for the two different agents, both biological in nature and presumably showing similar kinematics, remains the same. Therefore it might well be that visual features of agents are as important as action goal for modulating action observation activity, at least within the core “mirror” area F5c. We do not know whether manipulation concerned with the type of model would have produced similar results in other areas, with or without “mirror” properties, given that in this study the investigation was restricted to the ventral premotor cortex and nearby prefrontal regions by means of regions of interest (ROI) analyses (Nelissen et al., 2005).

Here we capitalize on the above mentioned findings to investigate for the first time whether in humans, as happens in monkeys, the action observation system or part of it is differentially activated depending on the type of view irrespective of action goal. This is a reasonable question to ask considering that a number of fMRI studies in humans have shown that action observation in humans evokes widespread frontal activation, including that of premotor area 6 and of prefrontal areas 44 and 45 which may modulate, as reported in monkeys (Nelissen et al., 2005) depending on the type of view (for review see Turella et al., 2009). Further, because ‘mirror’ activity in humans is detected across a number of areas which exceed those classically considered as ‘mirror’ (Gazzola et al., 2007a, Gazzola et al., 2007b, Gazzola and Keysers, 2008), it may be relevant to test the possible differences related to the type of view at whole-brain level in terms of action observation and execution overlapping activity.

Therefore, here we asked the same individual to observe a grasping action performed either by a fully visible model or by a hand alone (action observation conditions) and to perform a visually-guided grasping action (action execution condition) while scanned. These data may allow us to identify overlapping areas for both the observation and the execution of hand actions and how such activity might be modulated by the type of model.

Section snippets

Subjects

Seventeen paid right-handed volunteers (10 female, mean age 27.8 years, range 21–39) were recruited for the present study. They all had normal or corrected to normal vision and had no history of neurological problems. Informed consent was obtained from all subjects before the testing session in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki. One subject was discarded due to head motion exceeding 3 mm (voxel size).

Apparatus

We used a custom-built MRI-compatible apparatus consisting of two main parts (Fig. 1A

Main effect of type of observed task

As shown in Table 1, the t-contrast testing “action observation” against “static control”, (Model grasping + Hand alone grasping) > (Model static + Hand alone static), showed differential activation within the so-called “action-observation system” comprehending premotor, temporal and parietal cortices (Buccino et al., 2001, Pierno et al., 2006, Gazzola et al., 2007a, Gazzola et al., 2007b, Gazzola and Keysers, 2008). Activation map related to action observation is overlaid on the mean image of the

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether in humans different areas belonging either to the action observation or to a potential “mirror” system were differentially activated depending on the type of view. As outlined below, our results demonstrate a broad network of areas involved in both action observation and execution. We did find evidence that such network was activated both by a hand or a model acting, and that there was no modulation due to the visual properties of the

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dr. Christoph Braun und Jürgen Dax for constructing the MRI-compatible apparatus. The authors also thank Mathias Röger for helping with data acquisition, Sarah Mang and Hubertus Becker for helping with the Matlab programming and Caterina Ansuini for helpful comments on previous versions of this manuscript. This study was supported by a grant from the Italian Ministry of University and Research to U.C.

References (79)

  • GazzolaV. et al.

    The anthropomorphic brain: the mirror neuron system responds to human and robotic actions

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • GazzolaV. et al.

    Aplasics born without hands mirror the goal of hand actions with their feet

    Curr. Biol.

    (2007)
  • GoldmanA.I. et al.

    Simulation, mirroring, and a different argument from error

    Trends. Cogn. Sci.

    (2005)
  • GrèzesJ. et al.

    Activations related to 'mirror' and 'canonical' neurons in the human brain

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • HagbergG.E. et al.

    Improved detection of event-related functional MRI signals using probability functions

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • Johnson-FreyS.H. et al.

    Actions or hand-object interactions? Human inferior frontal cortex and action observation

    Neuron

    (2003)
  • Kuhtz-BuschbeckJ.P. et al.

    Brain activity is similar during precision and power gripping with light force: an fMRI study

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • LotzeM. et al.

    fMRI evaluation of somatotopic representation in human primary motor cortex

    Neuroimage

    (2000)
  • MaykaM.A. et al.

    Three-dimensional locations and boundaries of motor and premotor cortices as defined by functional brain imaging: a meta-analysis

    Neuroimage

    (2006)
  • MorinO. et al.

    What is “mirror” in the premotor cortex? A review

    Neurophysiol. Clin.

    (2008)
  • NakaharaK. et al.

    Exploring the neural basis of cognition: multi-modal links between human fMRI and macaque neurophysiology

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2007)
  • NicholsT. et al.

    Valid conjunction inference with the minimum statistic

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • OrbanG.A. et al.

    Comparative mapping of higher visual areas in monkeys and humans

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2004)
  • PeelenM.V. et al.

    Patterns of fMRI activity dissociate overlapping functional brain areas that respond to biological motion

    Neuron

    (2006)
  • PeraniD. et al.

    Different brain correlates for watching real and virtual hand actions

    Neuroimage

    (2001)
  • PicardN. et al.

    Imaging the premotor areas

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2001)
  • RizzolattiG. et al.

    Premotor cortex and the recognition of motor actions

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (1996)
  • SerenoM.I. et al.

    From monkeys to humans: what do we now know about brain homologies?

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2005)
  • ShmuelofL. et al.

    Dissociation between ventral and dorsal fMRI activation during object and action recognition

    Neuron

    (2005)
  • TurellaL. et al.

    Mirror neurons in humans: Consisting or confounding evidence?

    Brain Lang.

    (2009)
  • AstafievS.V. et al.

    Extrastriate body area in human occipital cortex response to the performance of motor actions

    Nat. Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • BegliominiC. et al.

    Comparing natural and constrained movements: new insights into the visuomotor control of grasping

    PloS ONE

    (2007)
  • BegliominiC. et al.

    Differential cortical activity for precision versus whole-hand visually guided grasping

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2007)
  • BirnR.M. et al.

    Event-related fMRI of tasks involving brief motion

    Hum. Brain Mapp.

    (1999)
  • BrettM. et al.

    Region of interest analysis using an SPM toolbox

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • BuccinoG. et al.

    Action observation activates premotor and parietal areas in a somatotopic manner: an fMRI study

    Eur. J. Neurosci.

    (2001)
  • Calvo-MerinoB. et al.

    Action observation and acquired motor skills: an FMRI study with expert dancers

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2005)
  • CastielloU.

    The neuroscience of grasping

    Nat. Rev. Neurosci.

    (2005)
  • CastielloU. et al.

    The cortical control of visually-guided grasping

    Neuroscientist

    (2008)
  • Cited by (38)

    • Fronto-parietal mirror neuron system modeling: Visuospatial transformations support imitation learning independently of imitator perspective

      2019, Human Movement Science
      Citation Excerpt :

      Moreover, the synaptic patterns were similar in the frontal and parietal MNS during both action observation and imitation along with the emergence of both view-independent and view-dependent fronto-parietal network neurons in the macaque monkeys that are reminiscent of the actual view-based mirror neurons. This was not preprogrammed per se but is also an emergent feature of our neural model which is critical since it is consistent with i) the well-established notion that MNS activity is similar during observation and execution of the same action (Arnstein et al., 2011; Gazzola & Keysers, 2009; Grèzes et al., 2003; Turella et al., 2009) and ii) the identification of view-based mirror neurons in primates (Caggiano et al., 2011). More importantly, beyond replicating the ratio between view-independent and view-dependent neurons in IFG, the neural model predicts that IPL and SPL/IPS would mainly include view-independent and view-dependent neurons, respectively.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text