Review
From perception to memory: Changes in memory systems across the lifespan

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.04.006Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We extend an interactive memory system model to incorporate memory changes across the lifespan.

  • The architecture and dynamics of memory systems become differentiated during development.

  • A dynamic shift toward favoring semantic memory occurs during aging.

Abstract

Human memory is not a unitary entity; rather it is thought to arise out of a complex architecture involving interactions between distinct representational systems that specialize in perceptual, semantic, and episodic representations. Neuropsychological and neuroimaging evidence are combined in support of models of memory systems, however most models only capture a ‘mature’ state of human memory and there is little attempt to incorporate evidence of the contribution of developmental and senescence changes in various processes involved in memory across the lifespan. Here we review behavioral and neuroimaging evidence for changes in memory functioning across the lifespan and propose specific principles that may be used to extend models of human memory across the lifespan. In contrast to a simplistic reduced version of the adult model, we suggest that the architecture and dynamics of memory systems become gradually differentiated during development and that a dynamic shift toward favoring semantic memory occurs during aging. Characterizing transformations in memory systems across the lifespan can illustrate and inform us about the plasticity of human memory systems.

Introduction

The notion that there are multiple memory systems in the human brain is widely accepted and supported by neuropsychological, computational, and neuroimaging data (see reviews in Schacter and Tulving, 1994, Squire, 2009). Just as memory is not a single faculty of the mind, the developmental and senescence changes in various processes involved in memory are also not uniform. In this paper we aimed to apply and extend a framework that captures the complex architecture and interactions between memory systems (PIMMS; Henson and Gagnepain, 2010) to the human lifespan. The PIMMS framework follows earlier accounts of memory in proposing three memory systems: perceptual, semantic, and episodic. However, it diverges from earlier accounts by highlighting the predictive interactions between systems as the general principle of operation between (and within each of) the systems.

In this paper we argue that the framework suggested by Henson and Gagnepain (2010) is useful for guiding a life-span perspective on memory, as it incorporates behavioral data and points to likely candidates for the corresponding neural architecture. We focus on explicit forms of memory, including encoding, recognizing, and recalling aspects of past experience in a conscious manner. In doing so, we acknowledge that we are leaving out important developmental changes in implicit forms of memory (Thomas et al., 2004). It is also beyond the scope of this review concerning potential differences during encoding and retrieval in the interaction between multiple memory systems. We review findings from age-comparative studies on encoding or retrieval by describing which process they focus on but without making further differentiation in developmental effects for encoding and retrieval.

We adopt the premise that memory serves a predictive function (cf. Schacter and Addis, 2007), a notion that is rarely examined within memory development and aging. Within the PIMMS model, the ‘predictive’ function refers more specifically to the idea that higher-level systems predict the activity in lower-level systems for basic perception. Later on, we discuss how the notion of ‘prediction’ can be extended to include more abstract conceptual knowledge that serves to guide behavior over time. In general, prediction-error-driven plasticity is a property of the brain that likely undergoes changes across the lifespan and may have broad implications for cognitive functioning. Research in recent years have capitalized on advancement in neuroimaging methodologies and yielded a growing evidence of changes in memory systems during development and in aging. This paper takes the viewpoint of the framework of interactive memory systems for organizing this wealth of evidence. We aimed to extend the notions of this framework to capture the dynamics and plasticity of changes in memory processes that occur during child development and aging. A unified framework across the life span, we believe, will not only help understanding the changes during development and aging, but will have implications for better characterization of the framework as applied to the more ‘stable’ form of the network during adulthood.

The perceptual, semantic, and episodic systems within the PIMMS framework are distinguished primarily by their representational content and assumed computational principles (see Fig. 1, middle panel). At the lowest level, the perceptual system extracts and represents features of incoming information from the environment. The semantic system records combinations of perceptually defined features that repeatedly co-occur in the environment and supports familiarity as a retrieval mechanism (Cowell et al., 2010, Murray et al., 2007, Rogers et al., 2004). At the apex of the hierarchy, the episodic system records events defined by a feature at a given context (i.e., background where the feature occurred), or co-occurrence of two or more unrelated features. It is assumed that the hippocampus is a key region of the episodic system and supports recollection as a retrieval mechanism, given its central role in binding mechanisms. In contrast, it is assumed that the perirhinal cortex is a key region of the semantic system (see extension of the semantic system in Section 2.3.2), whereas the more posterior cortices (e.g., the ventral visual pathway in the occipitotemporal cortex or the auditory pathway in the lateral temporal cortex) are key regions of the perceptual system. The proposed role of the perirhinal cortex as a key region in the semantic system is supported by its involvement in familiarity-based processes (Ranganath et al., 2004) and its apparent content-specificity for mnemonic processing of objects compared to scenes (Staresina et al., 2011, Watson and Lee, 2013).

The PIMMS framework fosters the notion that there is a high degree of recurrent interaction across memory systems and neural regions. It is assumed that feedback from one system predicts the activity in lower systems in the hierarchy. Feed-forward flow of information, on the other hand, transmits the difference between such top-down predictions and the current bottom-up input. For example, a representation of the current spatial context in the hippocampal system (e.g., entering a bathroom) may predict items that are likely to appear in that context. This is carried out by providing feedback to the semantic system and activating representations for certain familiar items (e.g., toothbrush, towel, etc.), which in turn guides activity in the ventral visual and auditory pathway. The purpose of such recurrent interactions is to minimize prediction error (cf. Bar, 2009, Friston, 2010). The difference between the feedback predictions and the forward transmission of sensory evidence is eventually minimized, while the system settles into perception of a specific object. In line with the Bayesian brain hypothesis (Knill and Pouget, 2004), the PIMMS framework assumes that the brain operates with the inherent tendency to attempt to predict its surrounding environment. Prediction errors are generated when there is a mismatch between the prediction and the immediate context, and serve to update the internal system to help improve predictions in the future. Larger residual prediction errors (after perception/retrieval has occurred) entail greater synaptic change, which will also lead to more successful encoding. Prediction error thus serves as a general process enabling the operation of memory systems and interaction between systems.

The PIMMS model focuses on interactions between the hippocampus, perirhinal cortex, and the ventral visual system for the purpose of predictive memory for item categories. Kroes and Fernandez (2012) advanced the notion of predictive memory to higher conceptual abstract knowledge, which arises from extracting regularities across diverse experiences. This process is assumed to achieve through dynamic interactions between the hippocampus and neocortex, including the medial prefrontal cortex (PFC). Across episodes, the hippocampus allows the integration, separation, and comparison of information from distributed brain regions, while the medial PFC integrates abstract representations across modalities with behavioral output (see also Roy et al., 2012) and represents semantic knowledge (Binder et al., 2009). With this, it is assumed that episodic memory gives rise to abstract knowledge that is akin to semantic memory. Such a notion converges with the complementary learning systems framework, which holds that the hippocampus learns rapidly using separated representations to encode the details of specific events, while the neocortex has a slow learning rate and uses overlapping distributed representations to extract the general statistical structure of the environment (McClelland et al., 1995). A recent extension of the complementary learning systems incorporates the role of the hippocampus in generalization (Kumaran and McClelland, 2012), achieved by incorporating recurrence flow within the hippocampal system and between the hippocampus and neocortex.

Therefore, the neural architecture of memory systems essentially includes the prefrontal cortex, both medial and lateral aspects. The prefrontal cortex has been implicated in memory functioning, through neuropsychological evidence of patients with lesions to the prefrontal cortex showing deficits in aspects of episodic memory (Janowsky et al., 1989, Schacter et al., 1984), and through its role in cognitive control. In particular the lateral PFC (including DLPFC, VLPFC, and anterior PFC) subserves goal-directed control functions that support the encoding of discrete memory traces, and the subsequent strategic retrieval and evaluation of stored representations (Simons and Spiers, 2003). More recently, van Kesteren et al. (2012) proposed a framework that relates the medial temporal lobe (MTL) and mPFC during memory processing of information as a function of its congruency with existing information represented in the neocortex (i.e., prior knowledge). This framework emphasizes the interaction between the MTL and mPFC, such that the mPFC detects the congruency of new information with existing information in neocortex. Only when there is low congruency will the MTL be involved in binding the elements of new information into a representation.

At this point we have summarized the guiding architecture and computational principles of the predictive memory systems model. With these in mind, we now turn to discuss the development of memory systems across child development, following by aging.

Section snippets

Memory systems’ development from childhood to adulthood

Children's memory improves as they grow, and there is general agreement that some aspects develop throughout childhood to adulthood, while others show little developmental trends past early childhood (Brainerd et al., 2004, Ghetti and Angelini, 2008, Ofen et al., 2007, Picard et al., 2012, Shing et al., 2008). A common notion in characterizing dissociations in the development of memory is between developments of memory that is rich in details such as the one used in episodic memory and during

Behavior – from perception to memory

At the perceptual level, processing of basic visual and auditory attributes deteriorates with age (e.g., Faubert, 2002). Alterations in the central processing of visual and auditory information have been shown to account for age-related variance on a broad array of higher-order cognitive tests (Baltes and Lindenberger, 1997, Lindenberger and Baltes, 1997). These findings suggest the importance of taking into consideration age alteration in basic perceptual processing when investigating

Memory systems across the lifespan

In our review of the literature in development and aging, we compare each developmental stage to the adult conceptualization of the memory systems according to the PIMMS model. To summarize our extension of the model, we posit that young children rely on rudimentary forms of perceptual, semantic and episodic systems, supported by the posterior cortices and perirhinal cortex of the MTL. Through development, higher-level abstract knowledge as well as top-down control supported by the frontal

Questions for future research

In addition to the points we discussed above, there are further issues that are important to be considered in future research of lifespan changes in the memory systems. We now focus our discussion on two main issues, as presented in the following sections.

Concluding remarks

In this review we take the challenge of incorporating behavioral and neuroimaging data from across the lifespan into current conceptualization of memory systems. In doing so we find that although memory functioning in children and older adults is impaired compared to young and middle aged adults, there is little symmetry in the likely causes of these differences. During development, memory systems may be less differentiated and the dynamics of their operation may prefer perceptual to semantic

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank N. Raz, U. Lindenberger for productive discussions that informed the framework presented in this paper, and R. Serota for constructive comments on previous versions of the paper.

References (136)

  • D. Friedman et al.

    Changes in familiarity and recollection across the lifespan: an ERP perspective

    Brain Res.

    (2010)
  • K.J. Friston et al.

    Dynamic causal modelling

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • S.A. Gelman et al.

    Categories and induction in young children

    Cognition

    (1986)
  • E.S. Ghatala et al.

    Developmental changes in incidental memory as a function of processing level, congruity, and repetition

    J. Exp. Child Psychol.

    (1980)
  • J.S. Janowsky et al.

    Source memory impairment in patients with frontal lobe lesions

    Neuropsychologia

    (1989)
  • S.S. Jones et al.

    The place of perception in children's concepts

    Cogn. Dev.

    (1993)
  • F.C. Keil et al.

    Two dogmas of conceptual empiricism: implications for hybrid models of the structure of knowledge

    Cognition

    (1998)
  • D.C. Knill et al.

    The Bayesian brain: the role of uncertainty in neural coding and computation

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2004)
  • W. Koutstaal et al.

    Gist-based false recognition of pictures in older and younger adults

    J. Mem. Lang.

    (1997)
  • M.C. Kroes et al.

    Dynamic neural systems enable adaptive, flexible memories

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • R. La Joie et al.

    Differential effect of age on hippocampal subfields assessed using a new high-resolution 3 T MR sequence

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • K.R. Laws et al.

    A meta-analytic review of category naming in Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • S.-C. Li et al.

    Aging cognition: from neuromodulation to representation

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2001)
  • U. Lindenberger

    Lifespan theories of cognitive development

  • J.M. Mandler et al.

    Developmental changes in picture recognition

    J. Exp. Child Psychol.

    (1978)
  • A. Maril et al.

    Event congruency and episodic encoding: a developmental fMRI study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2011)
  • V. Menon et al.

    Maturation of medial temporal lobe response and connectivity during memory encoding

    Brain Res. Cogn. Brain Res.

    (2005)
  • M. Moscovitch et al.

    The cognitive neuroscience of remote episodic, semantic and spatial memory

    Curr. Opin. Neurobiol.

    (2006)
  • S.G. Mueller et al.

    Selective effect of Apo e4 on CA3 and dentate in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease using high resolution MRI at 4 T

    Neuroimage

    (2008)
  • S.G. Mueller et al.

    Measurement of hippocampal subfields and age-related changes with high resolution MRI at 4 T

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2007)
  • L. Nadel et al.

    Memory formation, consolidation and transformation

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • L. Nyberg et al.

    Memory aging and brain maintenance

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2012)
  • N. Ofen

    The development of neural correlates for memory formation

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2012)
  • C. Ranganath et al.

    Medial temporal lobe activity associated with active maintenance of novel information

    Neuron

    (2001)
  • C. Ranganath et al.

    Dissociable correlates of recollection and familiarity within the medial temporal lobes

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • N. Raz et al.

    Trajectories of brain aging in middle-aged and older adults: regional and individual differences

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • H.P. Bahrick

    Semantic memory content in permastore: fifty years of memory for Spanish learned in school

    J. Exp. Psychol. Gen.

    (1984)
  • P.B. Baltes

    Theoretical propositions of life-span developmental psychology: on the dynamics between growth and decline

    Dev. Psychol.

    (1987)
  • P.B. Baltes et al.

    Emergence of a powerful connection between sensory and cognitive functions across the adult life span: a new window at the study of cognitive aging?

    Psychol. Aging

    (1997)
  • M. Bar

    The proactive brain: memory for predictions

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B: Biol. Sci.

    (2009)
  • J.R. Binder et al.

    Where is the semantic system? A critical review and meta-analysis of 120 functional neuroimaging studies

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2009)
  • C.J. Brainerd et al.

    Behavioral measurement of remembering phenomenologies: so simple a child can do it

    Child Dev.

    (2004)
  • C.J. Brainerd et al.

    Developmental reversals in false memory: a review of data and theory

    Psychol. Bull.

    (2008)
  • C.J. Brainerd et al.

    AreYoung children susceptible to the false-memory illusion?

    Child Dev.

    (2002)
  • Brehmer, Y., Li, S.-C., Mueller, V., von Oertzen, T., Lindenberger, U., 2007. Memory plasticity across the life span:...
  • R. Cabeza

    Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: the HAROLD model

    Psychol. Aging

    (2002)
  • R.B. Cattell

    Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action

    (1971)
  • X.J. Chai et al.

    Scene complexity: influence on perception, memory, and development in the medial temporal lobe

    Front. Hum. Neurosci.

    (2010)
  • C.Y.P. Chiu et al.

    Making memories: a cross-sectional investigation of episodic memory encoding in childhood using fMRI

    Dev. Neuropsychol.

    (2006)
  • R.A. Cowell et al.

    Components of recognition memory: dissociable cognitive processes or just differences in representational complexity?

    Hippocampus

    (2010)
  • Cited by (0)

    1

    Equal contribution.

    View full text