Elsevier

Journal of Veterinary Behavior

Volume 10, Issue 5, September–October 2015, Pages 407-412
Journal of Veterinary Behavior

Research
Do cats (Felis catus) predict the presence of an invisible object from sound?

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.06.002Get rights and content

Abstract

Recognizing invisible entities from auditory information is advantageous to animals in various situations including predator avoidance and foraging. In 2 experiments we asked whether cats could predict the presence of an unseen object on hearing noise it made, based on a causal-logical rule. After observing an experimenter shaking an opaque container for 15 seconds (observation phase), the cats freely explored the environment for 15 seconds (response phase). Experiment 1 tested 3 conditions. In the first, “contingent noise” condition, the object inside the container made a rattling noise when shaken. In the second, “irrelevant noise” condition, white noise accompanied the shaking action. In the third, “no noise” condition, the shaking action was silent. Experiment 2 tested a “noncontingent noise” condition, in which the rattling noise and movement of the container were out of synchrony. In both experiments cats looked at the container for longer in the “contingent noise” condition than the other conditions. These results suggest that cats used a causal-logical understanding of auditory stimuli to predict the presence of invisible objects. This ability may be related to the ecology of cats' natural hunting style.

Introduction

Information obtained via the sensory organs is often ambiguous or fragmentary. For example, an animal hunting in the bush by sight might hear only the noise the prey makes. In this case inferring the presence of a prey from the noise would be advantageous to the hunter's survival. Similarly, potential prey may be more likely to survive if they can predict the presence of predators from indirect clues such as odor and noise.

Inferential reasoning refers to the ability to use available information to draw conclusions about circumstances that are not directly observable (Heimbauer et al., 2012). Call (2004) explored this ability in great apes. He presented apes with 2 opaque cups, one of which they knew to be baited. The apes were given visual or auditory cues about the contents of both cups (full information) or only one of the cups (partial information) before making a choice. The subjects were able to see the contents of the cups in a visual domain test and to hear a rattling noise when the cup was shaken in an auditory domain test. The latter test required an understanding of the causal-logical rule between the noise and movement of the containers. In contrast to the full visual information task on which all apes succeeded, fewer subjects passed the auditory tests even with full information (16.6% (2/12), 50% (2/4), 0% (0/6), and 62.5% (5/8) of chimpanzees, bonobos, orangutans, and gorillas, respectively). Similar results have been obtained in other nonhuman primate species: 0% (0/8) in rhesus macaques (Petit et al., 2015), 33% (7/21) in olive baboons (Schmitt and Fischer, 2009, Petit et al., 2015), 50% (2/4) in lemurs (Maille and Roeder, 2012), and 30% (8/26) in capuchin monkeys (Sabbatini and Visalberghi, 2008, Paukner et al., 2009, Heimbauer et al., 2012), although 100% (8/8) of tonkean macaques succeeded in an auditory test (Petit et al., 2015). It appears surprising that this causal relationship is so poorly understood by primates.

Several researchers have related this poverty of causal understanding to the ecological importance of auditory information of each species (Maille and Roeder, 2012, Plotnik et al., 2014). Nonhuman primates are generally poor at auditory as opposed to visual tasks (Schmitt and Fischer, 2009). D'Amato and Salmon (1982) suggested that cats use auditory stimuli to locate prey, whereas primates often use sounds as cues to avoid rather than approach the source. Given that cats often use auditory cues when hunting (Turner and Meister, 1988), investigating cats' predictions about invisible objects from noise can contribute to understanding how ecological factors influence functional differences among sensory modalities.

It has been suggested that cats' causal-logical understanding in the physical domain is not sophisticated (Bradshaw, 2013). Whitt et al (2009) tested domestic cats on string-pulling tasks to explore their understanding of physical causality. After the cats were initially trained to pull a string to obtain a food reward, 3 tests were conducted. In “longer string” tests, cats were rewarded for choosing a baited string that was longer than the one used in training. In “parallel” and “crossed strings” tests, cats were required to choose between 2 strings, only one of which was baited. The cats failed to choose the baited string in both tests; no causal understanding was demonstrated. Bradshaw (2013) pointed out that string-pulling tasks lack ecological validity and that they may not be an appropriate test of cats' physical understanding. It may be advantageous to test cats' causal understanding using a different modality. Cats have a remarkable hearing ability (Bradshaw, 2013). For example, they have more than 20 muscles in their ear (see Tabor, 1983), and the audible range extends from 48 Hz to 85 kHz (Heffner and Heffner, 1985). We propose that the auditory modality may be more suitable test focus.

Here we present 2 experiments that investigated whether cats could show causal-logical understanding about the existence of an object inside an opaque container when they observe the container being moved accompanied by a rattling sound. We tested cats in 3 conditions in each of the 2 experiments. In experiment 1, we ran “contingent noise,” “irrelevant noise,” and “no noise” conditions. The experimenter shook the container repeatedly while the cats watched. In contingent noise condition, a block of wood inside the container made a rattling noise as it moved. In irrelevant noise condition, white noise was played during the movement of the container. In no noise condition, the experimenter shook the empty container. In experiment 2, “noncontingent noise” condition replaced “irrelevant noise” condition. In this new condition, the rattling noise was not synchronized with the motion of the container. We hypothesized that if cats form a representation of an invisible object from auditory stimuli, they would pay more attention to the contingent noise condition than to any other conditions.

Section snippets

Subjects

Thirty-eight domestic cats (24 males and 14 females) participated in experiment 1. Seventeen cats were kept at cat cafés1 and 21 were house pets. Eleven cats were pure breeds and 27 were mixed breeds. Their mean age was 3.1 years (range: 2-156 months). Details of the subjects are shown in Table S1. The cats were not deprived of food or water during the tests.

Apparatus and stimuli

We put a wooden block

Subjects

Thirty-two cats (18 males and 14 females) participated in experiment 2. Fifteen were kept at cat cafés and 17 were house pets. Six cats were pure breeds and 26 were mixed breeds. Their mean age was 3.4 years (range: 2-156 months). Details of the subjects are shown in Table S2. Thirteen of the subjects also participated in experiment 1. The interval between experiment 1 and experiment 2 was at least 2 months. The cats were never food or water deprived during the test.

Apparatus and stimuli

The apparatus was the same

General discussion

This study investigated whether cats could represent the existence of an unseen object in an opaque container from the rattling noise it made. We predicted that, if cats did so, they should show more interest in the container if the noise and motion of the container are physically congruent. Experiment 1 revealed that cats paid more attention to the container in the “contingent noise” condition, in which noise matched the motion of the container, than in the “no noise” condition. However, the

Conclusion

The present research investigated whether cats have a causal-logical understanding from sound. Through 2 experiments, we demonstrated that cats predict the existence of an unseen object from shaking movements accompanied by a concomitant sound. Although previous research showed that cats' causal-logical understanding was poor (Whitt et al., 2009), this study provided positive evidence of physical understanding in cats. Further research is needed to investigate more precisely the

Acknowledgments

This study was financially supported by the Grant-in-aide for Scientific Research No. 25240020 to Kazuo Fujita from the Japan Society for the Promotion of Science (JSPS). The authors thank all owners and cats who volunteered to participate in this study. The authors also thank James R. Anderson and Yuya Hataji for editing the article. Saho Takagi designed this study, conducted experiments, analyzed data, and drafted the article. Hitomi Chijiiwa, Minori Arahori, Mana Tsuzuki, and Ayami Hyuga

References (22)

  • L.E. Bahrick

    Infants' intermodal perception of two levels of temporal structure in natural events

    Infant. Behav. Dev.

    (1987)
  • R.S. Heffner et al.

    Hearing range of the domestic cat

    Hear. Res.

    (1985)
  • J.M. Plotnik et al.

    Thinking with their trunks: elephants use smell but not sound to locate food and exclude non rewarding alternatives

    Anim. Behav.

    (2014)
  • J. Bradshaw

    Cat Sense: The Feline Enigma Revealed

    (2013)
  • J. Call

    Inferences about the location of food in the great apes (Pan paniscus, Pan troglodytes, Gorilla gorilla, and Pongo pygmaeus)

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (2004)
  • M.R. D'Amato et al.

    Tune discrimination in monkeys (Cebus apella) and in rats

    Anim. Learn. Behav.

    (1982)
  • C. Dumas

    Object permanence in cats (Felis catus): An ecological approach to the study of invisible displacements

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (1992)
  • L.A. Heimbauer et al.

    Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use positive, but not negative, auditory cues to infer food location

    Anim. Cogn.

    (2012)
  • A. Hill et al.

    Inferential reasoning by exclusion in children (Homo sapiens)

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (2012)
  • A. Maille et al.

    Inferences about the location of food in lemurs (Eulemur macaco and Eulemur fulvus): a comparison with apes and monkeys

    Anim. Cogn.

    (2012)
  • A. Paukner et al.

    Tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) spontaneously use visual but not acoustic information to find hidden food items

    J. Comp. Psychol.

    (2009)
  • View full text