Identification of a dispositional tendency to experience work–family spillover

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvb.2013.01.006Get rights and content

Abstract

Are individuals predisposed to experience work–family spillover? Despite theoretical relevance and practical implications related to this issue, research on this topic is scarce. With this in mind, we investigated if there is a dispositional tendency to experience work–family spillover using a nationally representative longitudinal sample. We present evidence that supports the existence of a disposition to spillover by demonstrating that (a) a dispositional factor model accounts for data better than other competing factor models, (b) the dispositional factor is stable over time at a ten-year follow up, and (c) the dispositional factor is distinct from Big-5 personality traits. Findings highlight the important role that disposition plays in reports of work–family spillover and the necessity to consider individual differences in future work–family theories.

Highlights

► Evidence for a dispositional tendency to experience work–family spillover is provided. ► A nationally representative longitudinal sample was used. ► A factor model that includes a dispositional factor accounts for data better than other competing factor models. ► The dispositional factor is stable over time at a ten-year follow up. ► The dispositional factor is distinct from Big-5 personality traits.

Introduction

Social trends such as the increase in the number of dual career couples (U. S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2011) and a growing number of employees who are responsible for the simultaneous care of aging parents and children at home (Neal & Hammer, 2007) have sparked substantial scholarly interest in the interdependency between the work and family domains (i.e., spillover). Spillover refers to bidirectional effects between work and family (i.e., work-to-family, family-to-work) that generate similarities between the two roles (Edwards & Rothbard, 2000). Existing research acknowledges positive (i.e., experiences from one domain facilitate performance in another domain) as well as negative (i.e., experiences from one domain inhibit the fulfillment of demands in another domain) spillover (Allen, 2012). The combination of direction and valence results in four focal constructs: work-to-family negative spillover (WFNS); work-to-family positive spillover (WFPS); family-to-work negative spillover (FWNS); and family-to-work positive spillover (FWPS).

Considerable research suggests that both positive and negative work–family spillover relate to various organizational outcomes (e.g., Allen et al., 2000, McNall et al., 2010). In light of this knowledge, a host of antecedents of spillover have been examined, with environmental antecedents (e.g., family and occupational/organizational factors) having received the most attention. A growing body of research, however, complements the extant literature by investigating dispositional variables as predictors of spillover (Allen, 2012, Hammer and Zimmerman, 2011). Recent findings indicate that various individual differences such as negative affect relate to the experience of positive and negative spillover in both directions (Allen et al., 2012, Michel et al., 2011). This is a welcome addition to the literature considering that work–family experiences are a result of the interaction between environmental and person factors (Grzywacz & Marks, 2000).

Among studies that emphasize individual differences, research by Hecht and McCarthy (2010) deserves emphasis due to its unique contribution of demonstrating evidence for dispositional spillover tendencies. By showing that such tendencies exist and might originate from person characteristics (e.g., psychological resources and appraisal style), it highlights the profound role of personal attributes in the experience of work–family spillover. Further, considering such dispositions allows a novel perspective to work–family spillover; propensities for interrole conflict and facilitation are thought to exist beyond facet-level spillover (WFPS, WFNS, FWPS, and FWNS).

Extending this stream of research, the objective of the current study is to determine if a dispositional tendency exists to experience work–family spillover in general. We build on the findings of Hecht and McCarthy by providing evidence for a broader disposition that encompasses not only the direction of the spillover, but also the valence of the spillover. That is, we demonstrate that there is a dispositional tendency that links all four spillover constructs. Acknowledging this individual difference contributes to the work–family literature by providing an opportunity to better understand relationships among the four work–family spillover constructs (e.g., Greenhaus and Powell, 2006, Sumer and Knight, 2001). If a disposition to spillover that relates to all types of spillover indeed exists, previously observed associations among the spillover constructs might have been inflated or attenuated as a function of this trait. Clear understanding of the relationships among focal constructs is fundamental for synthesizing knowledge and developing future theories.

Next, our research advances the work–family literature that has focused on the link between specific personality factors and spillover (e.g., Bruck and Allen, 2003, Wayne et al., 2004) by examining an individual difference that is distinct from frequently studied personality variables (e.g., the Big-5). In this manner, we also broaden the domain of individual differences by testing the notion that a unique dispositional tendency exists that underlies the experience of spillover regardless of its valence and direction. Finally, we add to the small number of studies that have examined different forms of work–family spillover longitudinally (e.g., Frone et al., 1997, Grandey et al., 2005) by demonstrating the stability of the disposition to spillover over a 10-year interval. Previous longitudinal work–family research has been based on relatively short time lags (see Frone et al., 1997 for an exception). Thus, little is known with regard to the stability of spillover over a long period of time. We extend our knowledge of the temporal variability of work–family spillover by providing evidence to support the notion that the level of spillover that an individual experiences might be consistent by virtue of disposition to spillover.

In the following sections, we test a series of research questions to provide evidence of a disposition to spillover. First, we examine whether considering this disposition uniquely contributes to our understanding of work–family spillover above and beyond the four existing constructs through comparisons of factor models. This follows the long-held tradition of establishing psychological constructs via factorial validation (Cronbach & Meehl, 1955). Apart from factorial validity, we further examine the temporal stability of the disposition to spillover over a 10-year interval with the expectation that individual dispositions are stable over time (Funder, 2001). Finally, we investigate discriminant validity of the disposition to spillover; specifically, we examine whether the proposed dispositional factor can be distinguished from Big-5 personality traits.

Section snippets

Disposition to spillover

Although the four work–family spillover constructs differ in direction and valence, they each represent interdependencies between work and family at their core. That is, spillover occurs when experiences (e.g., affect, values, skills, or behaviors) from one domain influence another domain, which results in similarities between the two domains (Edwards & Rothbard, 2000). Building on this fundamental characteristic of spillover, we theorize that the level of work–family spillover, regardless of

Factorial validity

One way to empirically validate the existence of a disposition to spillover is to establish its factorial validity. In organization research, individual differences such as general mental ability have been validated through the use of factor analytic models (for a review see Drasgow, 2003). We demonstrate a disposition to spillover in a similar fashion. First, we examined whether a dispositional model (Fig. 1A) that includes a dispositional factor in addition to the focal spillover constructs

Temporal stability

One defining characteristic of individual disposition is that it is relatively stable over time (Funder, 2001). Previous research on various dispositional constructs, such as intelligence (Sternberg, 1985) and personality (Costa & McCrae, 1995), supports this notion. Consequently, disposition to spillover is expected to be stable within individuals over time. Therefore, we hypothesize that the factorial validity of the dispositional model holds longitudinally.

Aside from factorial stability, we

Discriminant validity

For disposition to spillover to be a viable construct, it is important to demonstrate its distinctiveness from other existing individual differences (Campbell and Fiske, 1959, Cronbach and Meehl, 1955). We seek discriminant validity evidence by investigating whether disposition to spillover is distinct from the Big-5 personality variables (neuroticism, extraversion, openness to experience, conscientiousness, and agreeableness; Goldberg, 1990). Our choice of the Big-5 is based on the fact that

Sample

The Mid-life Development in the United States (MIDUS) is a national survey of psychological and social factors related to health and well-being that was administered to over 7000 Americans. The sample for the current study was limited to married and working individuals. The sample from the first wave (1994–1995) consisted of 2645 individuals who were 44.01 years old on average (SD = 10.51), and was fairly gender-balanced (44.7% females). The sample from the second wave (2004) consisted of 1486

Results

Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for the scale items are shown in Table 1. Hypothesis 1 proposed that the dispositional model would fit the data better than the discrete, valence, and directionality models of work–family spillover. Results supported this hypothesis (See Table 2). Although the dispositional model had TLI values slightly lower than .90, the other fit indexes showed that the model provided reasonable fit. Overall, the dispositional model fit substantially better

Discussion

The objective of this study was to provide evidence of the existence of disposition to work–family spillover. By investigating work–family spillover from an important but understudied perspective that emphasizes the role of dispositions, we make a key contribution to the literature. Our findings suggest that disposition to spillover is a stable individual difference that underlies the experience of work–family spillover and is distinct from the Big-5 personality constructs. This is consistent

References (60)

  • K.M. Shockley et al.

    When flexibility helps: Another look at the availability of flexible work arrangements and work–family conflict

    Journal of Vocational Behavior

    (2007)
  • J.H. Wayne et al.

    Considering the role of personality in the work–family experience: Relationships of the big five to work–family conflict and facilitation

    Journal of Vocational Behavior

    (2004)
  • J.S. Wiggins et al.

    Personality structure: The return of the Big Five

  • T.D. Allen

    The work–family interface

  • T.D. Allen et al.

    Consequences associated with work-to-family conflict: A review and agenda for future research

    Journal of Occupational Health Psychology

    (2000)
  • S. Aryee et al.

    Rhythms of life: Antecedents and outcomes of work–family balance in employed parents

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (2005)
  • B.E. Ashforth et al.

    All in a day's work: Boundaries and micro role transitions

    Academy of Management Review

    (2000)
  • R.F. Baumeister et al.

    Bad is stronger than good

    Review of General Psychology

    (2001)
  • S.M. Bianchi

    Family change and time allocation in American families

    The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science

    (2011)
  • D.T. Campbell et al.

    Convergent and discriminant validation by the multitrait–multimethod matrix

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1959)
  • P.T. Costa et al.

    Domains and facets: Hierarchical personality assessment using the revised NEO personality inventory

    Journal of Personality Assessment

    (1995)
  • L.I. Cronbach et al.

    Construct validity in psychological tests

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1955)
  • F. Drasgow

    Intelligence and the workplace

  • J.R. Edwards et al.

    Mechanisms linking work and family: Clarifying the relationship between work and family constructs

    Academy of Management Review

    (2000)
  • M.R. Frone

    Work–family balance

  • M.R. Frone et al.

    Antecedents and outcomes of work–family conflict: Testing a model of the work–family interface

    Journal of Applied Psychology

    (1992)
  • M.R. Frone et al.

    Relation of work–family conflict to health outcomes: A four-year longitudinal study of employed parents

    Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology

    (1997)
  • D.C. Funder

    The personality puzzle

    (2001)
  • L.R. Goldberg

    An alternative “description of personality”: The Big-Five factor structure

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1990)
  • L.R. Goldberg

    The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure

    Psychological Assessment

    (1992)
  • Cited by (23)

    • A theory of work-family conflict episode processing

      2019, Journal of Vocational Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given these four alternative appraisals of accountability, researchers must now ask, “How many employees report, unprompted, that they blame work or family domains/roles themselves, as is assumed in current survey measures of WIF and FIW?” Finally, regarding WFC stability over time, Allen et al. (2012) found that WFC has been positively related to traits like negative affect (cf. Cho, Tay, Allen, & Stark, 2013). We suspect that any trait-like stability in WFC should be the result of consistently carrying a WF structural discrepancy.

    • Gains and losses related to career transitions within organisations

      2014, Journal of Vocational Behavior
      Citation Excerpt :

      It might have been that time pressure could show its impact as a challenge stressor if performance as an outcome were to have been investigated. Work–home interferences, in contrast, seem to be a stressor that varies inter-individually, and not only depending on the work situation, because recent research has suggested that there is a disposition to experiences of work–family spillover (Cho, Tay, Allen, & Stark, 2013). Looking on the bright side, career transitions also result in better PJ fit.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text