The logic of animal intergroup conflict: A review

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.05.004Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Structured review of models of animal intergroup conflict behavior.

  • Biologically relevant modeling choices are highlighted.

  • Recent instructive empirical findings are pointed out.

Abstract

We review the literature on various approaches to modeling animal intergroup conflict behavior in theoretical biology, highlight the intricacies emerging in the process of adding due biological realism to such models, and point out recent empirical findings that can inspire future theorizing.

Introduction

More than two millennia ago, in his History of Animals, Aristotle observed that “There is enmity between such animals as dwell in the same localities or subsist on the same food. If the means of subsistence run short, creatures of like kind will fight each other” (Aristotle, 1984, p. 949). In his On the Origin of Species, Darwin even limned life itself metaphorically as the ‘struggle for existence’, acknowledging that all forms of life are inevitably entangled in conflicts (Darwin, 1859). While the horrible events of the first half of the 20th century temporarily led to a focus of prominent European biologists on the more peaceful sides of animal behavior (Lorenz, 1963, Tinbergen, 1968), the modern synthesis in evolutionary biology (Huxley, 1942), with its emphasis on the ‘egoistic gene’ (Dawkins, 1976), has continuously upheld conflict as one of the most important themes in biology. Thus, it might not be a coincidence that the development of evolutionary game theory and its subsequent success in theoretical biology, economics and other disciplines began with a groundbreaking paper on ‘the logic of animal conflict’ (Maynard Smith and Price, 1973).

Conflicts between forms of (pre-)life, be they viruses, unicellular organisms, or more complex living beings, arise when one interferes in some way with another’s structural integrity, resource supply, growth, dispersal or reproductive interests. Obviously, such a broad definition of conflict comprises not only direct conflicts in which at least one party actively seeks to harm the other, but also indirect conflicts in which parties are negatively affected by each other’s mere existence or proliferation. Well-known examples of such indirect conflicts are intraspecific evolutionary arms races between trees competing for access to sunlight by growing taller (Dawkins and Krebs, 1979). Examples of direct conflicts, on the other hand, include interspecific predator-prey and host-parasite relations (Barbosa and Castellanos, 2005, Galvani, 2003, Schoener, 1983), chemical warfare between microbe species (Czaran et al., 2002), and intraspecific contests and potentially lethal fights (Enquist and Leimar, 1990), e.g. over reproductive access (Clutton-Brock and Huchard, 2013), parental investment (Trivers, 1974), or territory (Willems et al., 2013).

The central role of both direct and indirect conflicts in the evolution of virtually all forms of life has led to an abundance of respective empirical studies (for overviews see: Hardy and Briffa, 2013, Huntingford and Turner, 1987), and, when conceptualized as ‘the struggle for existence’, indirect conflict is implicitly present in practically all work in theoretical biology. Compared to this omnipresence of indirect conflict, intraspecific direct conflicts have remained a somewhat peripheral topic. Still, the respective literature has produced a number of seminal models, most of them closely tailored to distinct instances of aggressive behaviors observed in the field (for complementary reviews see: Kokko, 2013, Sherratt and Mesterton-Gibbons, 2013). One likely reason for the comparably high degree of segmentation in the theoretical literature on direct conflicts is that biological theorizing often quickly exchanges ideas and arguments with naturalist field work, as exemplified by the observational studies on non-lethal stag fights in deer that inspired Maynard Smith and Price’s models (Maynard Smith, 1974, Maynard Smith and Parker, 1976, Maynard Smith and Price, 1973), and vice versa (Clutton-Brock et al., 1979). The concentration of theoretical work in biology on select phenomena resulting from this close interplay of field and desk work, as well as the need for theories to remain testable by empiricists (Fawcett and Higginson, 2012), may thus have led to the current array of relatively specialized theories on animal conflict behavior.

The aim of this review is to present a structured overview of the existing theoretical literature on intergroup conflict behavior in biology. In doing so, we draw on instructive works by colleagues from theoretical biology and economics (Dechenaux et al., 2015, Kokko, 2013, Sheremeta, 2015; Sherratt and Mesterton-Gibbons, 2013). We follow their ‘top-down’ approaches of organizing their overviews according to general models of the incentive structures potentially faced by competing individuals and groups. Consistent with most biological models of conflict behavior, we use game theoretical terminology (Broom and Rychtar, 2013, Brown, 2016, Maynard Smith, 1982; for an alternative modeling approach see, e.g.: Santarlasci et al., 2014).

Two earlier, highly informative overviews of approaches to modeling animal conflict behavior already exist (Kokko, 2013; Sherratt and Mesterton-Gibbons, 2013). However, these mainly focus on dyadic and triadic conflict models. We complement them here by putting special emphasis on models of intergroup conflicts.

Inevitably, our review cannot do justice to the vast body of empirical literature on animal conflict behavior (for overviews of this literature see: Hardy and Briffa, 2013, Huntingford and Turner, 1987). Additionally, although we include several methodologically instructive models tailored to ancestral human intergroup conflicts, we confine this review mostly to the study of non-human animal behavior (for comprehensive reviews of human intergroup conflict behavior see, e.g., Glowacki et al. (forthcoming); Böhm et al. (forthcoming); as well as the other papers collected in this issue; Keeley, 1996; and Gat, 2008). Thus, we hope that our review will be found useful by an interdisciplinary readership interested in theoretical insights into the logic and dynamics of animal conflict and also by theorists looking for a guide to the respective methodological toolbox used by theoretical biologists.

Section snippets

Approaches to modeling intergroup conflict in biology

As the classic Hawk-Dove game introduced by Maynard Smith and Price (1973) set the stage for the majority of subsequent theoretical approaches to studying conflict in biology, we will start out by briefly recapitulating its main characteristics. Subsequently, we will extend our formalization of the game to be applicable to intergroup conflicts and use this extended version of the game to structure our review of different modeling approaches to animal intergroup conflict behavior and dynamics.

In

Adding further biological realism

The potential extensions of intergroup conflict models outlined in Section 2 of course do not exhaust the set of factors potentially relevant in modeling animal conflict behavior, as they are limited to changes in assumptions about individuals’ characteristics and the strategic features of the conflicts in which they are engaged. Consequently, let us now turn to reviewing selected approaches for studying the evolutionary dynamics and modeling the structure of populations in which these

Instructive empirical works to inspire future theorizing

As Sections 2 and 3 have shown, the existing theoretical literature on animal intergroup conflicts is diverse. The works discussed above have identified a remarkable variety of assumptions and modeling components that sensitively affect a model’s dynamics and long-term predictions, including: (i) impact, (ii) contest success, and (iii) cost functions; (iv) sharing rules; (v) strategy revision protocols; (vi) meta-population and subgroup sizes; (vii) demographics; (viii) within-group

Discussion and outlook

In this paper, we outlined the existing theoretical literature on approaches to modeling animal intergroup conflict behavior in theoretical biology (Section 2), highlighted the intricacies emerging in the process of adding due biological realism to such models (Section 3), and pointed out recent empirical findings that can inspire future theorizing (Section 4); see Table 3 for a synopsis.

In summary, we have seen that a plethora of models of animal intergroup conflict behavior exists using

Funding

SG was partially supported by the U.S. Army Research Office under grant number W911NF-14-1-0637 and by the National Science Foundation under NSF Award #DBI-1300426.

Acknowledgements

We thank Luke Glowacki, Kelly Rooker, Erik Kimbrough and Roman Sheremeta, and our anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on earlier versions of this paper as well as Kevin Laughren for generating Fig. 1. This work was assisted through participation in the “Evolution & Warfare” Investigative Workshop at NIMBIOS, sponsored by the National Science Foundation through NSF Award #DBI-1300426, with additional support from The University of Tennessee, Knoxville.

References (143)

  • J. García et al.

    Evolution of parochial altruism by multilevel selection

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (2011)
  • P. Hammerstein

    The role of asymmetries in animal contests

    Anim. Behav.

    (1981)
  • D.D. Johnson et al.

    Fight the power: Lanchester's laws of combat in human evolution

    Evol. Hum. Behav.

    (2015)
  • G.A. Kaiping et al.

    Cooperation and punishment in community-structured populations with migration

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2016)
  • K.A. Konrad et al.

    Evolutionarily stable in-group favoritism and out-group spite in intergroup conflict

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2012)
  • S. Lessard

    Long-term stability from fixation probabilities in finite populations: new perspectives for ESS theory

    Theor. Popul. Biol.

    (2005)
  • B. Majolo et al.

    The effect of intergroup competition on intragroup affiliation in primates

    Anim. Behav.

    (2016)
  • A.C. Markham et al.

    Intergroup conflict: ecological predictors of winning and consequences of defeat in a wild primate population

    Anim. Behav.

    (2012)
  • J. Maynard Smith

    The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflicts

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (1974)
  • K. McComb et al.

    Roaring and numerical assessment in contests between groups of female lions, Panthera leo

    Anim. Behav.

    (1994)
  • M. Mesterton-Gibbons et al.

    How residency duration affects the outcome of a territorial contest: complementary game-theoretic models

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2016)
  • M. Mesterton-Gibbons et al.

    Models of coalition or alliance formation

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2011)
  • K.M. Page et al.

    Unifying evolutionary dynamics

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (2002)
  • G.A. Parker

    Assessment strategy and the evolution of fighting behaviour

    J. Theor. Biol.

    (1974)
  • P. Abbot et al.

    Inclusive fitness theory and eusociality

    Nature

    (2011)
  • E.S. Adams et al.

    Lanchester's attrition models and fights among social animals

    Behav. Ecol.

    (2003)
  • Aristotle

    The Complete Works of Aristotle: The Revised Oxford Translation, Bollingen Series Lxxi, 2

    (1984)
  • T.J.M. Arseneau-Robar et al.

    Female monkeys use both the carrot and the stick to promote male participation in intergroup fights

    Proc. R. Soc. B–Biol. Sci.

    (2016)
  • P. Barbosa et al.

    Ecology of Predator-prey Interactions

    (2005)
  • J.L. Barker et al.

    Asymmetry within social groups: division of labour and intergroup competition

    J. Evol. Biol.

    (2016)
  • T.P. Batchelor et al.

    Fight tactics in wood ants: individuals in smaller groups fight harder but die faster

    Proc. Royal Soc. B–Biol. Sci.

    (2011)
  • C.T. Bergstrom et al.

    On the evolution of behavioral heterogeneity in individuals and populations

    Biol. Philos.

    (1998)
  • J.M. Biernaskie et al.

    Ecology and multilevel selection explain aggression in spider colonies

    Ecol. Lett.

    (2016)
  • R. Böhm et al.

    The psychology of intergroup conflict: a review of theories and measures

    J. Econ. Behav. Org.

    (2017)
  • J.L. Bower

    The occurrence and function of victory displays within communication networks

  • S. Bowles

    Did warfare among ancestral hunter-gatherers affect the evolution of human social behaviors?

    Science

    (2009)
  • H. Brandt et al.

    Punishing and abstaining for public goods

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2006)
  • M. Broom et al.

    Game-Theoretical Models in Biology, Chapman & Hall/CRC Mathematical and Computational Biology

    (2013)
  • J.S. Brown

    Why Darwin would have loved evolutionary game theory

    Proc. R. Soc. B–Biol. Sci.

    (2016)
  • R. Bruintjes et al.

    Out-group threat promotes within-group affiliation in a cooperative fish

    Am. Nat.

    (2016)
  • K.A. Cassidy et al.

    Group composition effects on aggressive interpack interactions of gray wolves in Yellowstone National Park

    Behav. Ecol.

    (2015)
  • J.-K. Choi et al.

    The coevolution of parochial altruism and war

    Science

    (2007)
  • S.M. Chowdhury et al.

    A generalized Tullock contest

    Public Choice

    (2011)
  • T.H. Clutton-Brock et al.

    Social competition and selection in males and females

    Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B

    (2013)
  • M. Cords

    Variable participation in the defense of communal feeding territories by blue monkeys in the Kakamega Forest, Kenya

    Behaviour

    (2007)
  • M.C. Crofoot et al.

    Cheating monkeys undermine group strength in enemy territory

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2012)
  • T.L. Czaran et al.

    Chemical warfare between microbes promotes biodiversity

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2002)
  • C. Darwin

    On the Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection: Or: The Preservation of Favoured Races in the Struggle for Life

    (1859)
  • R. Dawkins et al.

    Arms races between and within species

    Proc. R. Soc. B–Biol. Sci.

    (1979)
  • R. Dawkins

    The Selfish Gene

    (1976)
  • Cited by (48)

    • Determinants of hyena participation in risky collective action

      2023, Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text