Research report
Evaluating the unique contribution of intolerance of uncertainty relative to other cognitive vulnerability factors in anxiety psychopathology

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jad.2013.05.063Get rights and content

Abstract

Background

Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is a cognitive vulnerability factor associated with a wide range of anxiety psychopathology. Other cognitive vulnerabilities such as anxiety sensitivity (AS), distress tolerance (DT), and discomfort intolerance (DI) have also been investigated as constructs of interest across anxiety disorders. As researchers increasingly uncover viable transdiagnostic vulnerabilities to anxiety, it becomes important to determine the degree of overlap between these constructs.

Methods

The present studies examined the unique relationships between IU, other vulnerability factors (AS, DT, and DI) and social anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and worry symptoms across two nonclinical samples (n=217, n=241).

Results

Findings were highly consistent across samples. IU was significantly associated with anxiety symptoms in all analyses, even when accounting for other transdiagnostic risk variables. Anxiety sensitivity, was uniquely related to social anxiety and obsessive-compulsive symptoms in all analyses, but was related to worry in only one study. Distress tolerance was only uniquely associated with worry. Discomfort intolerance was not uniquely related to the anxiety symptoms in any analyses.

Limitations

Future research should attempt to replicate the findings in a clinical population and utilize a longitudinal design.

Conclusions

The robust and incremental relationships between IU and anxiety symptoms suggests the potential benefit of targeting IU in the context of transdiagnostic anxiety treatments.

Introduction

Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is an individual difference variable that has emerged in recent research as a construct of interest within anxiety psychopathology. IU is defined as “a cognitive bias that affects how a person perceives, interprets, and responds to uncertain situations on a cognitive, emotional, and behavioral level” (Dugas et al., 2004). Individuals high in IU experience the possibility of a future negative event as threatening and unacceptable, regardless of the probability of the event actually happening (Dugas et al., 2001). Generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) and worry were the first forms of anxiety psychopathology thought to be associated with IU (Dugas and Ladouceur, 2000, Holaway et al., 2006). As a central tenet in their cognitive model of GAD, Dugas and Robichaud (2007) conceptualize IU as a dispositional characteristic of individuals at risk for GAD. Within this model, those high in IU interpret ambiguous situations as more threatening relative to those low in IU. Due to the inherent uncertainty present in everyday life, IU is seen as a contributing factor to the avoidance and chronic worry seen in GAD. More recently, IU has been associated with a wide range of anxiety conditions including obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD; Holaway et al., 2006), social anxiety symptoms (Boelen and Reijntjes, 2009, Carleton et al., 2010), hoarding symptoms (Oglesby et al., 2012), as well as depression (McEvoy and Mahoney, 2011). Within these disorders, IU is proposed to be a vulnerability factor contributing to increased avoidance behaviors, such as worry and compulsive checking in GAD and OCD respectively. In addition, given the recent research linking IU to a myriad of anxiety and depressive disorders, IU has been proposed as an important transdiagnostic maintaining factor (McEvoy and Mahoney, 2011).

The interest in IU as a transdiagnostic construct is recent, but extensive work exists for other cognitive vulnerability factors for anxiety. Anxiety sensitivity (AS) is a well-established construct referring to the fear of the consequences of anxiety-related sensations (Reiss and McNally, 1985). Research has established AS as a risk factor for anxiety disorders and found that individuals with anxiety disorders have higher AS than individuals without an anxiety disorder (Kearney et al., 1997, Rabian et al., 1993, Taylor et al., 1992). Distress tolerance (DT) is an additional variable of interest in anxiety disorder research. DT refers to an individual's ability to experience and endure negative emotional states (Simons and Gaher, 2005). Individuals that are low in DT display a decreased capacity to tolerate negative emotions, whereas individuals that are high in DT display an increased capacity to tolerate negative emotions. Low DT has been associated with a variety of negative outcomes such as anxiety and depressive symptoms, eating psychopathology, and problematic substance use (Anestis et al., 2007, O'Cleirigh et al., 2007, Simons et al., 2005). Similar to DT, discomfort intolerance (DI) is a construct used to measure the ability to tolerate uncomfortable physical sensations (Schmidt et al., 2006a, Schmidt et al., 2006b). Previous research has suggested that DI may be an additional risk factor for anxiety psychopathology (Schmidt et al., 2006a, Schmidt et al., 2006b, Schmidt et al., 2007).

Recent work has examined the relationships between cognitive vulnerability factors. Bernstein et al. (2009) proposed a hierarchical model of AS, DT, and DI. According to their model, AS and DT are distinct yet related lower order factors of a common higher order affect tolerance and sensitivity factor, while DI is a distinct construct. Another study found AS to be a subfactor of DT within a higher order affect tolerance factor, with DI being part of a distinct higher order factor of physical intolerance (Mitchell et al., 2013). Confirmatory factor analysis revealed that AS and IU are better modeled as separate, correlated factors (r=0.68) rather than loading on a single higher order factor (Carleton et al., 2007). These results suggest that AS and IU are related yet distinct constructs. Other investigators have posited that AS may function only as a mediator of the relationship between negative affect and certain anxiety symptoms, while IU explains additional independent variance above and beyond negative affect in certain anxiety symptoms (Norton and Mehta, 2007, Norton et al., 2005).

A concern in the development of treatments targeting cognitive vulnerability factors is the degree of overlap between these constructs and specific anxiety symptoms. One study found that AS and IU are independently related to neuroticism (Sexton et al., 2003), a personality trait associated with anxiety disorders (Barlow, 2002). Studies have also revealed that IU predicts significant variance in social anxiety symptoms and worry above and beyond AS and neuroticism (Boelen and Reijntjes, 2009, Carleton et al., 2010, Dugas et al., 2001). However, there are no studies to date that investigate the unique contribution of IU across different anxiety disorder symptoms above and beyond the effects of AS, DT, and DI.

The present study seeks to examine the unique relationship between IU and symptoms of social anxiety disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, and generalized worry controlling for the effects of AS, DT, and DI. The main hypothesis for the study is that IU will show a unique relationship with social anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and worry symptoms above and beyond the contributions of AS, DT, and DI.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants consisted of 217 college students from a large university in the Southern United States. Participants were primarily female (69.1%) with ages ranging from 17 to 26 (M=18.85, SD=1.38). Eighty percent of the participants were Caucasian, 9% African American, 3% Asian, and 7% Other (e.g., bi-racial).

Anxiety sensitivity

The Anxiety Sensitivity Index-3 (ASI-3) was used to measure fear of anxiety related sensations (Taylor et al., 2007). In the present study, the ASI-3 demonstrated excellent internal

Sample descriptives

The means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations for all variables used in the current analyses can be found in Table 1. The mean DIS, DTS, and ASI-3 scores were comparable to those found in other reports utilizing non-clinical samples (Medley, 2013, Schmidt et al., 2006a, Schmidt et al., 2006b, Timpano et al., 2009). However, the mean PANAS negative affect, IUS, SIAS, non-hoarding OCI-R, and PSWQ total scores were slightly below those found in other reports utilizing non-clinical

Study 2

In order to validate the findings from Study 1, we attempted to replicate these findings in a separate sample. Additionally, in this sample we controlled for trait anxiety rather than negative affect to determine whether these relations continue to hold in the context of another broad measure of anxiety. This provides a more stringent test of the of the relationships between IU and social anxiety, OCD, and worry symptoms by controlling for anxiety per se, rather than the broader construct of

Participants

Participants consisted of 241 college students from a large university in the Southern United States. Participants were primarily female (76.8%). Age information was not available but students were primarily freshman (72.2%). Seventy eight percent of the participants were Caucasian, 6.6% were African American, 3.7% were Asian, and 7.9% identified as other (e.g., bi-racial).

Anxiety sensitivity (ASI-3)

See previous description of measure. In the present study, the ASI-3 demonstrated very good internal consistency (α=0.85).

Discomfort intolerance (DIS)

Sample descriptives

The means, standard deviations, and intercorrelations for all variables used in the current analyses can be found in Table 2. The mean DTS, IUS, non-hoarding OCI-R and PSWQ scores were comparable to those found in other reports utilizing non-clinical samples (Oglesby et al., 2012, Timpano et al., 2009). The mean STAI-T, SIAS, and ASI-3 scores were slightly lower than those found in other non-clinical samples (Hofmann et al., 2009, Medley, 2013, Timpano et al., 2009). However, the mean DIS total

Discussion

In support of our initial hypothesis, findings indicate that IU is robustly related to social anxiety, obsessive-compulsive, and worry symptoms after accounting for the other cognitive vulnerability factors. By controlling for negative affect and trait anxiety in the different models we can be more confident that these effects are not simply due to an underlying anxious temperament. Furthermore, consistency of these findings across two large, independent samples suggests that these results are

Role of funding source

This research was not funded by any source.

Conflict of interest

The authors of this manuscript do not have any actual or potential conflicts of interest to report or disclose.

Acknowledgments

The authors of this manuscript do not have any acknowledgments to publish.

References (45)

  • T. Meyer et al.

    Development and validation of the Penn State Worry Questionnaire

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1990)
  • C. O'Cleirigh et al.

    Does distress tolerance moderate the impact of major life events on psychosocial variables and behaviors important in the management of HIV?

    Behavior Therapy

    (2007)
  • A. Pertusa et al.

    Refining the diagnostic boundaries of compulsive hoarding: a critical review

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2010)
  • J.F. Samuels et al.

    Prevalence and correlates of hoarding behavior in a community-based sample

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2008)
  • N.B. Schmidt et al.

    Discomfort intolerance: evaluation of a potential risk factor for anxiety psychopathology

    Behavior Therapy

    (2007)
  • N.B. Schmidt et al.

    Discomfort intolerance: development of a construct and measure relevant to panic disorder

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2006)
  • N.B. Schmidt et al.

    Anxiety sensitivity: prospective prediction of panic attacks and axis I pathology

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2006)
  • S. Taylor et al.

    How does anxiety sensitivity vary across the anxiety disorders

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (1992)
  • M.D. Anestis et al.

    The multifaceted role of distress tolerance in dysregulated eating behaviors

    International Journal of Eating Disorders

    (2007)
  • D.H. Barlow

    Anxiety and its disorders: the nature and treatment of anxiety and panic

    (2002)
  • R.N. Carleton et al.

    Anxiety sensitivity and intolerance of uncertainty: requisites of the fundamental fears?

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)
  • M.J. Dugas et al.

    Intolerance of uncertainty and worry: investigating specificity in a nonclinical sample

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2001)
  • Cited by (81)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text