Elsevier

Cortex

Volume 49, Issue 6, June 2013, Pages 1553-1565
Cortex

Special issue: Research report
Anosognosia, autobiographical memory and self knowledge in Alzheimer's disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2012.09.006Get rights and content

Abstract

This article explores the relationship between lack of awareness of neuropsychological deficit, also termed anosognosia, and loss of self knowledge in Alzheimer's disease (AD). Specifically, it considers the hypothesis that anosognosia in AD can in part be explained by a loss of mnemonic ability in which knowledge about self-ability is degraded. To ground this hypothesis, we review evidence suggesting failure to update personal knowledge concerning task efficacy, loss of recollection with relative amplification of semanticization processes and loss of an updated representation of the self. We present a theoretical formulation as to how the features of memory impairment in AD may contribute to anosognosia, incorporating these notions in a reformulation of the Cognitive Awareness Model.

Introduction

Lack of awareness about cognitive deficits or illness is a conspicuous feature of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and can be considered to be a direct result of the disease process (Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007), although moderated by psychosocial factors (Clare et al., 2012a, 2012b). Hence, in the context of a neurobiologically determined disorder, loss of awareness in AD can reasonably be given the term ‘anosognosia’ following Babinski's (1914) original application in relation to lack of knowledge of neurological disease. Anosognosia in AD has been shown to affect multiple facets of cognition and behaviour, including not only perceptions of illness but awareness of deficit relating to cognitive functions, such as memory, executive functioning, language and social cognition, and also judgements about behavioural efficacy, including self care ability, social interaction, driving and instrumental activities of daily living (Clare et al., 2012a; Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004; Nelis et al., 2011). In other words, unlike some other forms of anosognosia, such as that found for example in cortical blindness (Anton, 1898) or hemiparesis (Babinski, 1914), the main feature in AD is the lack of specificity of anosognosia in relation to overall function. People with AD almost universally underestimate the presence of neuropsychological deficit, for example, minimising difficulties or attributing changes to normal ageing.

Techniques for measuring anosognosia in AD have enabled quantification of severity levels, which in turn have proved useful for experimental investigation. One approach is directly appraising levels of awareness of illness or cognitive deficit by interviewing the person with AD (e.g., Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007; McGlone et al., 1991; Reed et al., 1993). This approach can explore, for example, beliefs about illness status, which can be independent of awareness of neuropsychological impairment. A second approach is to use a questionnaire (or structured interview) in which the person with dementia rates their everyday abilities or symptoms and this is compared to a parallel informant rating. In this case, the questionnaire can be used to target particular aspects of awareness, for example, memory functioning (Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004). Although subjective memory complaints concerning cognitive dysfunction, including memory, are relatively common in older people (Jonker et al., 2000), a main finding in AD, even in the early stages, is the tendency to overestimate ability in comparison to informants; discrepancies between the two sources of information, termed the subjective rating discrepancy (SRD) (Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007), or lack of correlation quantifies the extent of anosognosia. Finally, experimental studies can be conducted in which the person with AD is provided with a task likely to result in failure and have to judge their performance, either by making a prediction or estimating performance on completion. The difference between performance and judgement gives rise to an objective-judgement discrepancy (OJD) measure. This approach measures the more immediate response to failure, and is potentially useful in determining the extent to which online error detection is compromised in AD.

Of note, whatever method is used, group studies of AD detect overall loss of awareness of illness or deficit, even if the results from the different techniques are not necessarily highly correlated (Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007). Such approaches have shown that anosognosia occurs in the early stages of AD (Clare et al., 2012b), and even in the prodromal stages, as in mild cognitive impairment (MCI), although there is evidence for variability in the latter group, with some patients showing overestimation of subjective memory complaints, and other evidence for underestimation of memory impairment (Jungwirth et al., 2004; Roberts et al., 2009; Tremont and Alosco, 2011; Vogel et al., 2004), such that reliance on subjective complaints of symptoms may be suspect with regards to diagnosis. Longitudinal studies suggest that in early AD levels of anosognosia are relatively stable (Clare et al., 2012b), with evidence that with further increases in the severity of dementia anosognosia becomes more prominent but in a non-linear fashion (e.g., Aalten et al., 2006; Mograbi et al., 2012a).

Theoretical accounts of anosognosia in AD are likely to be complex and take into account the different processes associated with awareness. These include, for example, monitoring the level of success or failure on everyday tasks, translating the outcome of monitoring into decisions about current function and then further abstraction relevant information into more permanent ‘self-belief.’ With AD, different accounts have been produced, including those suggesting that lack of awareness is due to a failure of monitoring and control processes (e.g., Cosentino et al., 2007; Souchay, 2007), the effects of emotional dysregulation when engaging in self-monitoring (Rosen, 2011), impaired decision-making processes (e.g., Agnew and Morris, 1998; Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004), or due to the inability to consolidate new personal information (Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004). Recently, the ‘Petrified Self’ hypothesis has been developed by Mograbi et al. (2009) in which it has been proposed that unawareness in AD may be differentially associated with specific aspects of memory function, tapping into episodic/semantic and remote/recent memory distinctions.

In this paper we propose an elaboration of the hypothesis in which anosognosia is caused primarily by a decline in specific mnemonic processes leading to a loss of personal knowledge. Central to this hypothesis is the notion that consolidation of personal information is a pre-requisite for the development of self-identity over the lifespan. Here, awareness of functional ability is considered to be a special case of personal trait judgement that concerns personal efficacy. Furthermore, it is proposed that awareness involves the shaping of individual knowledge and behaviour, with continual modification to maximise personal efficacy and minimise experience of failure (Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004). In other words, the mechanisms that facilitate awareness of functional ability should be considered developmentally, invoking memory and acquired representation, and teleonomically, invoking purposefulness.

As a cause of anosognosia, impairments in specific aspects of memory function may prevent the updating of the self-concept and result in the person not consolidating information concerning their reduction in neuropsychological function (Mograbi et al., 2009). In connection with this, Conway (e.g., Conway, 2005; Conway and Pleydell-Pearce, 2000) suggests the existence of a “working self”, which modulates encoding of new information based on goals and self-image. Here, memory provides continuity to the experience of selfhood by allowing storage of past information and the ability to project future scenarios (Addis and Tippett, 2004). It constraints what the self is, has been and can be (Conway, 2005). Evidence from the study of brain damaged patients has suggested that profound impairment in mnemonic function clearly result in alterations in identity and selfhood, but with relatively preserved personality (Corkin, 2002; Klein and Nicols, in press; Rosenbaum et al., 2005; Tulving, 1999).

Such research indicates that different forms of memory give specific contributions to the formation of a self-concept. For example, there is substantial evidence that trait judgements about self or others are based on semantic knowledge extracted from episodes or life experiences (Klein et al., 2002a, 2008). Furthermore, semanticized traits facilitate more rapid access than if judgements were to be made on the basis of episodic memory. Nevertheless, there is evidence that trait summaries are updated in an accumulative fashion, with updating associated with each new episode. The nature of trait judgement may depend on the amount of experience of a person, with cursory judgement based on recent behavioural examples and more extensive experience leading to more abstracted trait summaries (Sherman and Klein, 1994). In relation to the development of self knowledge, the long time span of experience suggests self trait summaries are not only strongly semanticized as personal semantic knowledge but through a lengthy neural consolidation process (Tulving, 1985a, 1985b; Tulving et al., 1988). Nevertheless, episodic memory also contributes to a sense of self, allowing autonoetic consciousness, time and travel and re-experiencing of details. The continuity and richness of self experience (i.e., the ability to integrate specific and detailed past and present selves) relies on episodic autobiographical memory, and recent evidence suggests this is one of the last cognitive abilities to appear during development and the first to be lost with ageing and amnesic conditions, such as in AD (Piolino et al., 2006).

In summary, the formation of self knowledge may start with recollective experiences and to be followed by semanticisation and this process can be considered in relation to self knowledge concerning personal efficacy, with semanticized trait information enabling a person to make predictions about task performance (Nelson and Narens, 1990) or facilitate evaluation of current performance in relation to past experience, including recalibrating future expectations (Moulin et al., 2000a).

To illustrate these processes as they might apply to awareness of failure, Table 1 provides schematic material relating to everyday activity, providing examples of individuals experiencing task failure. For the purposes of illustration, the protagonist fails in the activities, as might occur in AD, but the awareness processes are assumed to be working properly so as to indicate their mechanism. Take the car parking illustration as the example: the first stage is the experience of task failure in a familiar situation which is adequately monitored and gives rise to the realisation of failure. This is accompanied by specific memories that later on can be retrieved for further consideration in which further nuancing of the situation might occur by evaluating what happened in the particular context (the circumstances associated with failure to find the car park). The memory for the experience is then semanticised into a general understanding, in which with repeated negative experience personal knowledge about car parking experiences is coded more abstractly and then crystallised into a self-concept ‘I am now not so good at navigation.’ Note that these processes can occur in a reverse fashion with positive experiences or mastery of a situation, revising expectations upwards. In AD it is claimed in this paper that impairments in these consolidation processes in part give rise to loss of this recalibration and hence unawareness of neuropsychological deficit.

The mechanisms that support such processes have already been considered to some extent in an existing framework used to investigate anosognosia in AD, namely the Cognitive Awareness Model (CAM Fig. 1; Agnew and Morris, 1998; Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007; Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004; see also Ansell and Bucks, 2006). The essential features of this model are that incoming knowledge concerning task or activity performance is monitored by comparator mechanisms that contrast this information to that stored in a Personal Database (PDB), a specialised storage of information about current function. The output of the comparison leads to updating of the PDB if there is a mismatch between the two sources of data and the resulting information is released via a further mechanism, namely the Metacognitive Awareness System (MAS) to provide consciousness of decision making. In addition to sparing of error monitoring and detection, awareness can only be generated in the presence of multiple preserved memory abilities. For example, immediate error monitoring would depend on abilities such as iconic and working memory to hold information relating to failure for sufficient time to allow error evaluation. A core feature of this model is the PDB, which retains information about performance efficacy and is continually updated by experience; experiences of success or failure relating to personal efficacy should lead to a recalibration of information contained in the PDB (Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007). Furthermore the PDB is moderated by social and interpersonal influences, including aspects not considered in this article (see: Clare et al., 2012a). Previously we have argued that a range of neurocognitive factors relating to different facets of the model can produce anosognosia in AD, those concerned with memory or consolidation aspects giving rise to what we have termed mnemonic anosognosia (Agnew and Morris, 1998; Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007; Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004).

Section snippets

Anosognosia as a failure of memory consolidation

One of the main predictions of the CAM is that evaluation of current performance relies on the availability for recollection of previous information about personal abilities, i.e., a PDB about cognitive ability. Hence this article considers the hypothesis that anosognosia in AD can in part be explained by a loss of mnemonic ability in which knowledge about self-ability is limited to outmoded semantic knowledge. To substantiate this hypothesis, we explore the strands of evidence that suggest

Third-person perspective and awareness

It has been shown in the context of anosognosia for hemiplegia (Fotopoulou et al., 2009; Ramachandran and Rogers-Ramachandran, 1996) and also suggested in clinical settings in the case of dementia that unaware patients may be able to acknowledge deficits in others or in themselves when exposed to evidence from a third-person perspective. Although acknowledging deficits in others but not self may merely suggest the operation of self-serving biases in cognition (Kahneman and Tversky, 1972), it is

Summary

In summary, the evidence reviewed suggests retrieval difficulties and lack of consolidation of new material about self-ability in AD, which we hypothesise, is secondary to loss of autobiographical memory. Because of this, people with AD rely more heavily on remote personal semantics as the basis for evaluation of expected ability, without recollection of success or failure events. This leads to a stable but vague and temporally inaccurate sense of self-ability, with impoverished reflective

A revision of the CAM

In this section we present a revision of the CAM (Agnew and Morris, 1998; Hannesdottir and Morris, 2007; Morris and Hannesdottir, 2004). An important feature of anosognosia is its heterogeneous presentation, with various potential causes leading to this phenomenon and unawareness following lesions to different brain areas (Agnew and Morris, 1998). Thus, neurocognitive models of anosognosia must take into account its multiplicity, providing a comprehensive explanation of potential deficits

Levels of awareness and brain systems

The revised CAM model is essentially modular in that consideration of the mechanisms associated with types of anosognosic conditions tends to follow the progression of information processing from unimodal to heteromodal representation. This feature can also be expressed in terms of levels of awareness (see also Clare et al., 2011), as indicated by the model and also in Table 2. In summary, at the bottom level are predominately unimodal sensory and motor processes, with progression to

Evolution of deficit in AD and comparison with MCI

This article concerns genesis of anosognosia in AD and the putative causative factors. Nevertheless, the initial presentation or evolution of anosognosia in AD, in particular the prodromal phase as in MCI does not necessarily follow an even course. In particular, there is evidence that people with MCI are actually more likely to overestimate their complaints of cognitive impairment in everyday life, for example memory functioning, and appraisal of functioning is better correlated with objective

Conclusions

To summarise, different strands of evidence point to the potential link between unawareness in AD and loss of recollective ability and self knowledge. The studies reviewed suggest that AD patients have difficulties retrieving episodic detail and that most of their available memories about self-ability belong to more remote periods and are recalled in semanticized form, lacking autonoetic features. The evidence suggests impairment in consolidating information about performance failure in order

References (106)

  • M. Irish et al.

    Impaired capacity for autonoetic reliving during autobiographical event recall in mild Alzheimer's disease

    Cortex

    (2011)
  • A. Ivanoiu et al.

    Retrieval of episodic and semantic autobiographical memories in early Alzheimer's disease and semantic dementia

    Cortex

    (2004)
  • D. Kahneman et al.

    Subjective probability: A judgment of representativeness

    Cognitive Psychology

    (1972)
  • S.B. Klein et al.

    Is there something special about the self? A neuropsychological case study

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2002)
  • M.D. Kopelman

    Remote and autobiographical memory, temporal context memory and frontal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients

    Neuropsychologia

    (1989)
  • T. Leyhe et al.

    Impairment of episodic and semantic autobiographical memory in patients with mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • O. Meulenbroek et al.

    Autobiographical memory retrieval in patients with Alzheimer's disease

    NeuroImage

    (2010)
  • D.C. Mograbi et al.

    Anosognosia in Alzheimer's disease – The petrified self

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2009)
  • D.C. Mograbi et al.

    Emotional reactivity and awareness of task performance in Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2012)
  • C.J.A. Moulin et al.

    The effects of repetition on allocation of study time and judgements of learning in Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychologia

    (2000)
  • C.J.A. Moulin et al.

    Brain function during multi-trial learning in mild cognitive impairment: A PET activation study

    Brain Research

    (2007)
  • K.J. Murphy et al.

    Episodic, but not semantic, autobiographical memory is reduced in amnestic mild cognitive impairment

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • A. Perrotin et al.

    Metamemory monitoring in mild cognitive impairment: Evidence of a less accurate episodic feeling of knowing

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • R.S. Rosenbaum et al.

    The case of K.C.: Contributions of a memory-impaired person to memory theory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2005)
  • C. Souchay

    Metamemory in Alzheimer's disease

    Cortex

    (2007)
  • L.R. Squire

    Memory systems of the brain: A brief history and current perspective

    Neurobiology of Learning and Memory

    (2004)
  • M. Startup

    Awareness of own and others' schizophrenic illness

    Schizophrenia Research

    (1997)
  • E. Tulving et al.

    Priming of semantic autobiographical knowledge: A case study of retrograde amnesia

    Brain and Cognition

    (1988)
  • P. Aalten et al.

    Awareness and behavioral problems in dementia patients: A prospective study

    International Psychogeriatrics

    (2006)
  • D.R. Addis et al.

    Memory of myself: Autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer's disease

    Memory

    (2004)
  • S.K. Agnew et al.

    The heterogeneity of anosognosia for memory impairment in Alzheimer's disease: A review of the literature and a proposed model

    Aging & Mental Health

    (1998)
  • G. Anton

    Uber Herderkrankungen des Gehirnes, welche van Patienten selbst night wahrgenommen werden

    Weiner Klinische Wochenschrift

    (1898)
  • B.J. Baars

    In the theatre of consciousness: Global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness

    Journal of Consciousness Studies

    (1997)
  • M.J. Babinski

    Contribution à l'etude des troubles mentaux dans l'hémiplégie organique cerebrale (Anosognosie)

    Revue Neurologique

    (1914)
  • D. Bernsten et al.

    Emotionally charged autobiographical memories across the life span: The recall of happy, sad, traumatic and involuntary memories

    Psychology and Aging

    (2002)
  • A. Berti et al.

    Shared cortical anatomy for motor awareness and motor control

    Science

    (2005)
  • J.C. Cavanaugh et al.

    Metamemory: A critical examination

    Child Development

    (1982)
  • L. Clare et al.

    Awareness in Alzheimer's disease and associated dementias: Theoretical framework and clinical implications

    Aging & Mental Health

    (2011)
  • L. Clare et al.

    The influence of psychological, social and contextual factors on the expression and measurement of awareness in early-stage dementia: Testing a biopsychosocial model

    International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • L. Clare et al.

    Longitudinal trajectories of awareness in early-stage dementia

    Alzheimer Disease and Associated Disorders

    (2012)
  • L. Clare et al.

    ‘She might have what I have got’: The potential utility of vignettes as an indirect measure of awareness in early-stage dementia

    Aging & Mental Health

    (2012)
  • F. Clement et al.

    Cognitive complaint in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease

    International Journal of the Neuropsychological Society

    (2008)
  • M.A. Conway et al.

    The construction of autobiographical memories in the self-memory system

    Psychological Review

    (2000)
  • S. Corkin

    What is new with the amnesic patient HM?

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2002)
  • S.A. Davidoff et al.

    Effect of video self-observation on development of insight in psychotic disorders

    Journal of Nervous & Mental Disease

    (1988)
  • C. Derouesne et al.

    Decreased awareness of cognitive deficits in mild dementia of the Alzheimer's type

    International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (1999)
  • M. Donix et al.

    Overgenerality of autobiographical memory in people with amnestic mild cognitive impairment and early Alzheimer's disease

    Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology

    (2010)
  • L.M. Duke et al.

    Cognitive components of deficit awareness in Alzheimer's disease

    Neuropsychology

    (2002)
  • A.W. Ellis et al.

    Age of acquisition effects in adult lexical processing reflects loss of plasticity in maturing systems: Insights from connectionist networks

    Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (2000)
  • P. Fromholt et al.

    Autobiographical memory in normal aging and primary degenerative dementia (dementia of Alzheimer type)

    Journal of Gerontology

    (1991)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text