Elsevier

Computers & Education

Volume 95, April 2016, Pages 19-35
Computers & Education

The impact of digital devices vs. Pen(cil) and paper on primary school students' writing skills – A research review

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2015.12.001Get rights and content

Highlights

  • We reviewed 10 studies on early writing comparing pen and paper and digital tools.

  • These studies can be located within three theoretical perspectives.

  • The majority indicate that handwriting outperforms keyboarding in early writing.

  • Methodological flaws: inadequate control for experience; lack of nesting effects.

Abstract

In the light of the continuing digital revolution in education and learning in general, and in literacy instruction in particular, the purpose of this review is to assess the emerging literature on such digital writing tools as computers and tablets compared with traditional writing tools like pen(cil) and paper, on early writing outcomes among first writers. We limited our review to studies published in international peer-reviewed journals during the last decade, within different theoretical perspectives. We identified a relatively small number of studies that can be categorized, as qualitative studies applying a case study design or within-subject design, and as quantitative studies, either quasi-experimental or cohort studies. These studies can be located within three research perspectives: 1) cognitive psychology, 2) neuroscience and learning and 3) socio-cultural theoretical perspective. While findings across the three perspectives were inconsistent, they were rather consistent within each perspective. While studies with a cognitive psychological and those with neuroscience and learning perspective point in favor of handwriting, studies with a socio-cultural perspective rather point in favor of digital writing. The studies that used a cognitive psychology and neuroscience and learning approach applied quasi-experimental or cohort designs, while studies based on a socio-cultural perspective mainly were qualitative. When analyzing the studies regarding methodological quality we found three flaws: small sample size (of quantitative studies); a lack of nesting effects; and inadequately controlling for experience for early writing. Facing an interdisciplinary research topic in rapid development, we provide some implications for further research, and suggestions in particular in terms of methodological challenges.

Introduction

The continuing international public and scholarly debate dealing with the future of early literacy instruction, i.e. instruction in becoming a reader and a writer, in primary school, is highly polarized. On one side, proponents of digitalization in education emphasize the possibilities of tablets and keyboards in early writing instruction and the potential of such tools to motivate students, in particular struggling learners (Genlott and Grönlund, 2013, Trageton, 2012) and students with specific learning disabilities (e.g. Berninger, Nagy, Tanimoto, Thompson, & Abbott, 2015). A meta-analysis by Morphy and Graham (2012) for example found clearly positive effects of word processing programs on different writing outcomes, for weaker readers and writers.

On the other side, a growing literature has shown the importance of handwriting for early writers' further cognitive development (e.g., Dinehart, 2015). Handwriting appears to activate particular areas in children's brains more than other forms of fine motor manipulation tasks, like tracing or typing letters (e.g., James and Engelhardt, 2012, Kersey and James, 2013). Dinehart and Manfra (2013) found that handwriting skills more strongly could predict reading and math. They argue that writing letters by hand leads to the development of an “internal model of the alphabetic character” (ibd. 155), regarded as a foundation for further academic achievement in contrary to simply pressing on a keyboard. Writing movements themselves appear to be involved in the process of memorizing and the visual representation of letters, and thus, support the benefits of ‘motor practice’ (Vinter & Chartrel, 2010: 484) for early writing (e.g., Longcamp et al., 2003, Longcamp et al., 2005, Vinter and Chartrel, 2010). In sum, these studies defend the practice of handwriting in early school years, as handwriting skills clearly have shown to be positively associated with further academic skills.

With this and ongoing controversy about the future of literacy instruction at primary school as a backdrop, the purpose of this article is to review internationally-published research of the last decade on the impact of pen(cil) and paper compared to digital (writing) tools (keyboards or digital tablets) in early writing instruction, on early writing outcomes.

While most of the existing literature focuses on secondary school students (e.g. Clarke & Svanaes, 2012) or university students, (Fortunati and Vincent, 2014, Taipale, 2014), relatively little attention has been paid to primary school students' early writing instruction (see also Boscolo, 2008), literature comparing digital tools with pen and paper. Among the existing literature on early writing instruction, a great number of studies exclusively deal with handwriting instruction (see for review: Dinehart, 2015; see also Accardo et al., 2013, Berninger et al., 1992, Graham et al., 2000, Pontart et al., 2013). Studies on early literacy instruction, covering both reading and writing – have focused mostly on early reading and its importance for further learning and academic achievement (e.g. Hattie, 2009). According to the hypotheses of the Matthew effect in reading, further learning and academic outcomes will increase to larger proportions for strong early readers; this means an increasing gap in academic achievement over time between strong and weak readers and writers (Cipielewski and Stanovich, 1992, Cunningham and Stanovich, 1997, Stanovich, 1986). Thus, paying attention to early literacy skills is crucial to foster further learning and educational progress.

In line with scholars focusing on the importance of early reading, and in line with scholars focusing on the importance of early writing (e.g., Graham et al., 2015, Persky et al., 2003), we argue that early writing skills are a prerequisite for further learning and academic achievement, particularly in the digital age (e.g., Burnett and Merchant, 2015, Merchant, 2007, Juzwik et al., 2006), a discourse with a rather shifting focus from text reception to text production. To cite Berninger et al. (2015:154): ‘The Great Debate about Reading during the Industrial Age [ … ] has been replaced with a new debate about writing in [ … ] information age’.

As two intertwined categories of literacy, we can also understand reading and writing more widely as a social process. According to Scribner and Cole (1981: 236) literacy can be conceptualized as ‘a set of socially organized practices which make use of a symbol system and a technology for producing and disseminating it. [Thus,] [l]iteracy is not simply knowing how to read and write a particular script but applying this knowledge for specific purposes in specific contexts of use.’ Addressing social contexts, the wide research area of ‘New Literacies Studies' (Lankshear and Knobel, 2003, Marsh, 2007) address different perspectives of literacy, from more narrow concepts of literacy limited to the dualism between reading and writing (Barton, 2007) to broader concepts including for example visual and auditory elements distinguishing between different levels and conceptions (e.g., Kress & Van Leeuwen, 2001). In our review, some studies draw on such an approach, considering the complexity of literacy needs in the digital age (e.g., Burnett and Merchant, 2015, Moje et al., 2000). These studies (Hultin & Westman, 2013) are included in the section of socio-cultural studies. The scope of our review, however, is limited to a more narrow approach of early writing instruction.

Focusing on students in preschool and early grades in primary school – early writers – we include studies on the use of digital writing tools including keyboards and tablet computers with touch screen technology as an alternative to pen and paper (handwriting) in early writing instruction. Our perspective is comparative, with focus on the dualism between digital writing instruction vs. traditional writing instruction by using pen-and-paper devices. Thus, we will not include in our review studies that explicitly focus on ‘handwriting tools with new technologies’ (Onofrei, Smaranda, Ţicău, & Şoitu, 2014) or ‘pen-based systems’ (Djeziri, Guerfali, Plamondon, & Robert, 2002), in other words, hybrid writing tools that combine features of digital writing tools and ‘traditional’ tools like pen and paper as this is beyond the scope of this review.

Further, we look at early writing outcomes like letter writing, spelling and simple sentences, in addition to subjective outcomes (writing experience). As our focus is on first writers, we do not explicitly consider higher-level writing outcomes such as planning, revising and editing (Berninger & Chanquoy, 2012). As we review a relatively new emerging field, we will include both exploratory and hypotheses-testing studies to consider both objective and subjective writing outcomes of first writers.

The next section gives a brief overview over the topic of early writing instruction by using pen and paper vs. digital tools according to the theoretical perspective applied, followed by a brief section on the method used to review the included studies. The remaining sections present the results and discuss the main findings, and finally inform the wider debate on future studies on the effect of digital devices vs. pen and paper in early literacy instruction in formal and non-formal settings.

Section snippets

Background: studies on handwriting and digital writing

From earlier research on handwriting, the evidence clearly shows a positive association between good handwriting skills and different dimensions of academic achievement, such as reading (e.g. James & Engelhardt, 2012) and memorizing skills (Dinehart, 2015). These findings are supported by studies indicating weaker associations between typing and memorizing letter outcomes compared with handwriting (e.g. Longcamp et al., 2008). Thus, the value of these studies lies in the fact that they

Method

Our literature review includes studies dealing with early writing instruction, setting an upper limit at primary school students aged 12. Bearing in mind an increasing focus on preschool education (Weigel et al., 2010, Wells, 1985), we do not exclude studies of preschool children from the age of three and older. Moreover, we may assume that children of that age have developed some early writing skills (Longcamp et al., 2005), and a large proportion of children at that age have been exposed to

Results

First, we present a descriptive overview of the included studies categorized under the three theoretical perspectives presented above, according to main author name, year of publication, country, title and journal name, intervention and comparator, study design and main outcomes. Second, we review the selected studies in detail, with a specific eye on study design and methods. Third, we present an overall analysis of the findings deriving from the review of the sample of our included studies

Synthesis of findings and discussion

Drawing on the controversial debate on the future of first-writing instruction in primary school, we have in this article reviewed international studies published during the last decade that investigated the impact of two different writing tools, pencil and paper vs. digital writing tools, or writing instruction modes, handwriting and typing, on early writing outcomes. Our overall aim was to paint a picture of a dynamic and developing topic over the last decade, to identify some general trends

Conclusion and implications for further studies

As demonstrated in this review, the benefits of replacing handwriting by typing in early writing instruction lack consistent evidence.

On the one side, earlier published studies, mostly within the field of cognitive psychology, show an advantage in using traditional writing tools like pen and paper compared with typing in early writing instruction (Connelly et al., 2007, Crook and Bennett, 2007), which might be due to children receiving more handwriting instruction than typing instruction in

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank their colleagues at NIFU Cay Gjerustad, Bjørn Stensaker, Vibeke Opheim and Rune Reiling, and Nalini Silje Løver, master's student at the University of Oslo, for useful comments and recommendations. The article was funded by NIFU's internal grant (project number 12820530-67).

References (82)

  • G. Kraaykamp

    Literary socialization and reading preferences. Effects of parents, the library, and the school

    Poetics

    (2003)
  • M. Longcamp et al.

    Visual presentation of single letters activates a premotor area involved in writing

    Neuroimage

    (2003)
  • M. Longcamp et al.

    The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing

    Acta Psychologica

    (2005)
  • J.C. Read

    A study of the usability of handwriting recognition for text entry by children

    Interacting with Computers

    (2007)
  • S. Taipale

    The affordances of reading/writing on paper and digitally in Finland

    Telematics and Informatics

    (2014)
  • S. Taipale

    Bodily dimensions of reading and writing practices on paper and digitally

    Telematics and Informatics

    (2015)
  • W. Van Peer

    Literary socialization in the family: a state of the art

    Poetics

    (1991)
  • A. Vinter et al.

    Effects of different types of learning on handwriting movements in young children

    Learning and Instruction

    (2010)
  • D. Barton

    Literacy: An introduction to the ecology of written language

    (2007)
  • V.W. Berninger et al.

    Comparison of pen and keyboard transcription modes in children with and without learning disabilities

    Learning Disability Quarterly

    (2009)
  • V. Berninger et al.

    What writing is and how it changes across early and middle childhood development. A multidisciplinary perspective

  • V.W. Berninger et al.

    Teaching spelling and composition alone and together: implications for the simple view of writing

    Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2002)
  • V. Berninger et al.

    Lower-level developmental skills in beginning writing

    Reading and Writing. An Interdisciplinary Journal

    (1992)
  • R.W. Black

    Fanfiction writing and the construction of space

    E-Learning and Digital Media

    (2007)
  • R.W. Black

    Online fan fiction and critical media literacy

    Journal of Computing in Teacher Education

    (2009)
  • P. Boscolo

    Writing in primary school

  • M.-L. Bosse et al.

    Lexical orthography acquisition: is handwriting better than spelling aloud?

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2014)
  • J. Bruner

    The culture of education

    (1996)
  • C. Burnett et al.

    The challenge of 21st-century literacies

    Journal of Adolescent & Adult Literacy

    (2015)
  • C.A. Christensen

    Relationship between orthographic-motor integration and computer use for the production of creative and well-structured written text

    British Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2004)
  • L. Clarke et al.

    Young pupils', their teacher's and classroom assistants' experiences of iPads in a Northern Ireland school:“Four and five years old, who would have thought they could do that?”

    British Journal of Educational Technology

    (2015)
  • B. Clarke et al.

    One-to-one tablets in secondary schools: An evaluation study stage one

    (2012)
  • V. Connelly et al.

    A comparison of keyboarded and handwritten compositions and the relationship with transcription speed

    British Journal of Educational Psychology

    (2007)
  • C. Crook et al.

    Does using a computer disturb the organization of children's writing?

    British Journal of Developmental Psychology

    (2007)
  • A. Cunningham et al.

    Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability 10 years later

    Developmental Psychology

    (1997)
  • L.H. Dinehart

    Handwriting in early childhood education: current research and future implications

    Journal of Early Childhood Literacy

    (2015)
  • L.H. Dinehart et al.

    Associations between early fine motor development and later math and reading achievement in early elementary school

    Early Education and Development

    (2013)
  • A.H. Dyson

    Playing with textual toys: Popular culture and childhood writing

  • J. Fitzgerald et al.

    Reading and writing relations and their development

    Educational Psychologist

    (2000)
  • L.S. Flower et al.

    A cognitive process theory of writing

    College Composition and Communication

    (1981)
  • S.W. Freedman et al.

    Conceptualizing a whole-class learning space: a grand dialogic zone

    Research in the Teaching of English

    (2007)
  • Cited by (98)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text