Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 123, Issue 1, April 2012, Pages 174-179
Cognition

Brief article
Perception mirrors production in 14- and 18-month-olds: The case of coda consonants

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2011.12.001Get rights and content

Abstract

In a word learning experiment, 14- and 18-month-old infants are tested on their perceptual sensitivity to coda-consonant omissions. The results indicate that 14-month-olds are not sensitive to coda consonant omissions, showing a parallel with the omission of target coda consonants in early child language productions. At 18 months, infants are sensitive to coda-omission. The study strengthens the hypothesis that phonological wellformedness constraints influence infants’ speech processing in general, and might restrict what is stored in their initial lexical representations. A lexical representation lacking information on the target coda consonant is, in turn, a likely source for coda-omissions in production.

Introduction

When babies produce their first meaningful words, for some time they omit target coda consonants (e.g. Demuth, 1995, Fikkert, 1994, Ingram, 1978, Menn, 1976, Moskowitz, 1970, Smith, 1973). Examples from English (Menn, 1976) and Dutch (CLPF database: Fikkert, 1994, Levelt, 1994) are in (1):

In the literature, this type of omission is accounted for in terms of phonological wellformedness constraints on young children’s output forms: a syllable template that only allows C(onsonant)V(owel) sequences (Demuth, 1995, Macken, 1979, Menn, 1978), a coda-parameter with a default setting “no codas allowed” (Dresher and Kaye, 1990, Fikkert, 1994) or a no-coda constraint that is initially high-ranked in an optimality theoretic grammar (Levelt, Schiller, & Levelt, 2000).

In the above accounts it is, implicitly or explicitly, assumed that children’s underlying forms, i.e. the stored lexical representations, are segmentally detailed. The argument for this assumption is that infants are able to perceive phonetic detail from very early on (Smith, 1973, Smolensky, 1996). Indeed, in several studies infants have demonstrated sensitivity to mispronunciations (e.g. Bailey and Plunkett, 2002, Ballem and Plunkett, 2005, Mani and Plunkett, 2007, Swingley, 2003, Swingley, 2005, White and Morgan, 2008). However, since infants failed to do so in other studies (e.g. Pater et al., 2004, Stager and Werker, 1997, Van der Feest, 2007), in the present study the lexical representation of target coda consonants that infants construct during word-learning is investigated.

Jusczyk (1998) hypothesized that principles of phonological wellformedness may play a role in speech processing from the start, predisposing infants to attend to certain aspects of the speech signal. Jusczyk, Smolensky, and Allocco (2002) indeed found evidence for a perceptual equivalent for the phonological output constraint on nasal place assimilation, in 4,5- and 10-month-old infants.

In Jusczyk (1998) preliminary results are reported showing that 6-month-old infants have a perceptual bias for syllable structures that satisfy both the coda constraint and a constraint requiring onsets, i.e. CV-syllables. If this bias would also lead infants to parse CV-syllables from the acoustic signal during word-learning, it is conceivable that target coda consonants initially remain un(der)represented in the lexicon. In the present study the hypothesis is, thus, that infants’ lexical representations for target coda consonants remain heavily underspecified. Such incomplete representations could form the initial source for coda-deletions in early word productions.

Up until now, only a few mispronunciation studies have looked at coda consonants. Swingley (2005) tested mispronunciations in both onsets and codas of known words in a head-turn experiment with 11-month-olds. While he found a clear mispronunciation effect for onsets, the results for codas were less obvious; Infants preferred listening to familiar words over nonwords, but this preference went away when the familiar words’ codas were mispronounced. This suggests that infants knew the codas’ phonological features. On the other hand, infants did not prefer familiar words over altered-coda variants of those words, suggesting that they did not distinguish between correct and incorrect codas. Zamuner (2006) found similar negative results for coda-mispronunciations with 11-month-olds and mixed results with 16-month-olds. In Swingley (2009) onset and coda mispronunciations were tested again using a referential visual fixation task with 17-month-olds (mean age). For this age group he did find a mispronunciation effect for both onsets and codas. In a similar experiment with 14-month-olds by Langeslag, Altvater-Mackensen, and Fikkert (2008), however, no mispronunciation effect was found for stop-fricative mispronunciations in codas, while the effect was present for onsets. In general, then, younger infants, i.e. the 11- and 14-month-olds, seem to pay less attention to codas.

All of these studies on coda consonants tested the infant’s sensitivity to a segmental change. In the present study the perception and representation of coda consonants is considered from the perspective of the earliest word productions, and here target coda consonants are omitted, rather than changed. In the experiments described below, the coda in mispronounced test items is therefore omitted. In the control condition the target word has a CV syllable structure, and a coda is added in the mispronounced test item. Coda-addition never happens in early word-productions.

We tested 14-month-olds, since this age group often omits codas from target words in production. The prediction is straightforward: if 14-month-olds indeed build incomplete representations during word-learning, parsing CV structures from the acoustic input, they should be tolerant of coda-omissions in mispronounced items. We do expect sensitivity to coda-addition. One experiment was repeated with 18-month-olds, who do usually produce the codas of target words.1

If this entails that the coda-constraint on speech processing is lifted, they should be sensitive to coda-omission in the mispronounced test-item.

Section snippets

Experiment 1: 14-month-olds

Experiment 1 studied the sensitivity of 14-month-olds to coda information in a novel-word learning task with four conditions: the novel word either contained one of three different coda consonants, /t/, /s/, /k/, that would be omitted in the test phase, or it would end in a vowel, and a coda /t/ would be added.

Methods

The procedure and scoring were the same as in experiment 1, the stimuli were the same as described above for novel word /pat/.

General discussion

In the above experiments it was shown that 14-month-olds are not sensitive to mispronunciations involving coda-omission, while they are sensitive to a mispronunciation involving coda-addition. The 18-month-olds did show sensitivity to coda-omission.

Two conclusions are feasible. One is that 14-month-olds notice additions but not omissions in a general perceptual sense. A similar effect would then be expected in, for example, visual discrimination. However, in experiments on either numerical or

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank the babies who participated in this study and their parents, Marijn van ’t Veer, Monique Bisschop, Evelien de Later, Mirjam de Jonge and Renee Middelburg for running the experiments and coding the videos, Kees Verduin en Niels Schiller for their help with the statistics, and two anonymous reviewers for their valuable comments. This work is supported by a VIDI grant (276-75-006) from the Netherlands Organisation for Scientific Research (NWO).

References (40)

  • S. Antell et al.

    Perception of numerical invariance in neonates

    Child Development

    (1983)
  • D. Archangeli

    Aspects of underspecification theory

    Phonology

    (1988)
  • T. Bailey et al.

    Phonological specificity in early words

    Cognitive Development

    (2002)
  • K. Ballem et al.

    Phonological specificity in children at 1;2

    Journal of Child Language

    (2005)
  • Boersma, P., & Weenink, D. (2007). Praat: Doing phonetics by computer (Version 4.5.18) [Computer program]....
  • E. Coenen et al.

    Variation and assimilation in German: Consequences of assimilation for word recognition and lexical representation

    Language and Cognitive Processes

    (2001)
  • L. Cohen et al.

    Habit 2000: A new program for testing infant perception and cognition (Version 2.2.5c) [Computer software]

    (2000)
  • De Lacy, P. (2006). Markedness: Reduction and preservation in phonology. Cambridge Studies in Linguistics, 112....
  • K. Demuth

    Markedness and the development of prosodic structure

  • C. Fennell et al.

    Early word learners’ ability to access phonetic detail in well-known words

    Language and Speech

    (2003)
  • Cited by (9)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text