Tactile P300 to unpredictable electric shocks: Association with anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty, and neuroticism

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108094Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Examined tactile P300 to predictable and unpredictable shocks.

  • Measured anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty (IU), and neuroticism.

  • P300 enhanced to unpredictable relative to predictable shocks.

  • P300 enhancement associated with anxiety symptoms, IU, and neuroticism.

  • Attentional engagement to unpredictable threat heightened in anxiety phenomena.

Abstract

The no, predictable, and unpredictable threat (NPU-threat) task is an experimental paradigm that delineates the anticipation of predictable vs. unpredictable threat. The current literature suggests that heightened defensive motivation in anticipation of unpredictable threat is associated with anxiety disorders and increased symptoms. Few investigations have examined whether a heightened response to actual threat is also associated with anxiety-related phenomenology. The present study examined the relationship between the tactile P300 to shock delivery during the NPU-threat task and individual differences in anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty, and neuroticism. Overall, the tactile P300 was enhanced in response to unpredictable shocks relative to predictable shocks. Greater tactile P300 enhancement to unpredictable shocks was associated with greater anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty, and neuroticism. The present study suggests that temporal unpredictability enhances attentional engagement to threat, which is greater in individuals characterized by narrow and broad anxiety constructs.

Introduction

Aversive stimuli have been shown to impact neural and physiological indicators of emotional and motivational reactivity. For example, the perception of threatening and unpleasant visual stimuli increase the startle reflex (an indicator of defense motivation), corrugator activity (an indicator of negative affect), and both event-related potentials (ERPs) and neural response in a number of different brain regions, including the amygdala, hippocampus, and prefrontal cortex (Aldhafeeri, Mackenzie, Kay, Alghamdi, & Sluming, 2012; Blau, Maurer, Tottenham, & McCandliss, 2007; Bradley, Codispoti, & Lang, 2006; Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert, 1990; Lang, Greenwald, Bradley, & Hamm, 1993; Robinson, Charney, Overstreet, Vytal, & Grillon, 2012). Aversive imagery and scripts also impact reactivity, such that unpleasant, relative to pleasant, scenes potentiate the startle reflex, corrugator activity, and skin conductance response (an indicator of arousal) (Miller, Patrick, & Levenston, 2002). Finally, auditory and tactile stimuli, such as aversive sounds and CO2-enriched air, heighten corrugator activity and heart rate (Bradley, Zack, & Lang, 1994; Forsyth, Lejuez, & Carlos., 2000). Overall, aversive stimuli elicit robust neural and physiological responses that are consistent with enhanced emotional and motivational reactivity.

There are several conceptual and theoretical models that implicate increased reactivity to threat as a potential etiological mechanism of anxiety disorders and symptoms (Engel, 1977; Fanselow & Lester, 1988; Mogg, Mathews, & Weinman, 1987; Seligman & Binik, 1977). Indeed, there is ample evidence supporting these models across multiple methodologies (Grillon & Baas, 2003; Vaidyanathan, Patrick, & Cuthbert, 2009). For example, individuals with specific and social phobia have been shown to exhibit an enhanced startle reflex to fear-relevant stimuli (De Jong & Merckelbach, 1996; Globisch, Hamm, Esteves, & Öhman, 1999; Hamm, Cuthbert, Globisch, & Vaitl, 1997; McTeague et al., 2009). Additionally, individuals at risk for anxiety, such as children with a maternal history of an anxiety disorder, show potentiation of the startle reflex when viewing unpleasant images (Kujawa, Glenn, Hajcak, & Klein, 2015). Anxiety disorders and symptoms have also been associated with elevated ERPs indicating enhanced attention towards threat. A number of anxiety disorders, including panic disorder, social phobia, and specific phobia, as well as trait anxiety have been associated with an increased late positive potential (LPP) in response to negative and unpleasant stimuli (MacNamara & Hajcak, 2010; MacNamara, Ferri, & Hajcak, 2011; Michalowski et al., 2009; Miltner et al., 2005; Moser, Huppert, Duval, & Simons, 2008; Pauli et al., 1997). The increased neural and physiological reactivity observed across multiple disorders and traits suggests that it may reflect a transdiagnostic mechanism of anxiety disorders.

Research has begun to identify particular features of aversive stimuli that may contribute to increased emotional and motivational reactivity. Predictability is one characteristic that has been shown to impact reactivity1 . Unpredictability diminishes the ability to prepare for, respond to, or avoid danger and threat (Grupe & Nitschke, 2013). Thus, heightened emotional and motivational responding is adaptive in the context of unpredictable threat, but becomes maladaptive when the response exceeds the true danger of the threat (Rosen & Schulkin, 1998). A maladaptive response to ambiguous, uncertain, or unpredictable threat has been proposed as a key mechanism of multiple forms of psychopathology, most notably anxiety disorders (Carleton, 2016; Gentes & Ruscio, 2011).

Temporal predictability of threat has been demonstrated to be particularly effective in eliciting defensive responses in the laboratory relative to other features such as occurrence uncertainty (Bennett, Dickmann, & Larson, 2018). The impact of temporal predictability on emotional and motivational reactivity has often been studied using the no, predictable, and unpredictable threat (NPU-threat) task. In the NPU-threat task, participants complete three within-subject conditions; no aversive stimulus is delivered in the no threat (N) condition, a signaled threat is delivered in the predictable threat (P) condition, and an unsignaled threat is delivered in the unpredictable threat (U) condition (Schmitz & Grillon, 2012). Numerous studies have demonstrated that the startle reflex is potentiated in anticipation of both predictable and unpredictable threat relative to no threat, with more recent evidence suggesting that startle potentiation is greater in anticipation of unpredictable threat relative to predictable threat (Ferry & Nelson, 2020; Gorka, Lieberman, Shankman, & Phan, 2017; Nelson & Hajcak, 2017b). Previous literature has also demonstrated that the type of threat administered during the task may impact the measurable response and that electric shock is consistently shown to be a potent aversive stimulus (Ferry & Nelson, 2020; Grillon, Baas, Lissek, Smith, & Milstein, 2004; Nelson & Hajcak, 2017b).

A growing number of studies have examined the relationship between startle potentiation during the NPU-threat task and anxiety disorders, symptoms, and clinical traits. For example, increased startle potentiation in anticipation of unpredictable (but not predictable) threat has been associated with a number of different anxiety disorders and symptoms, including panic attacks and panic disorder (Burani & Nelson, 2020; Grillon et al., 2008, 2017; Lieberman, Gorka, Shankman, & Phan, 2017) and specific phobia and social phobia (Gorka et al., 2017; Nelson & Hajcak, 2017a). In addition, increased startle potentiation in anticipation of unpredictable threat has been linked to clinical traits that are risk factors for the development of anxiety disorders, such as intolerance of uncertainty (IU) (Nelson & Shankman, 2011; Nelson, Liu, Sarapas, & Shankman, 2016). Finally, at least one investigation has found that increased startle potentiation in anticipation of unpredictable threat was associated with neuroticism (Craske et al., 2009). Overall, increased startle potentiation in anticipation of unpredictable threat has been associated with both narrow (disorders, symptoms) and broad (clinical and personality traits) individual difference measures of anxiety.

As previously mentioned, there is compelling evidence supporting increased emotional reactivity to threat as a potential mechanism of anxiety disorders and symptoms (Grillon & Baas, 2003; Vaidyanathan et al., 2009). Yet, few studies have examined whether this relationship is due to an element of uncertainty or unpredictability in the aversive stimulus. For example, it is possible that the content of aversive stimuli involves more uncertainty and unpredictability relative to neutral content, which at least partially contributes to the increased reactivity observed in individuals with anxiety disorders and symptoms. Uncertainty and unpredictability have both been shown to enhance ERP responses to the delivery of actual aversive stimuli. Indeed, pictures following uncertain relative to certain cues have been shown to elicit greater P2 and LPP responses in a community sample (Dieterich, Endrass, & Kathmann, 2016), and an enhanced P2, N2, and LPP in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder relative to a control group (Dieterich, Endrass, & Kathmann, 2017). A separate investigation found an enhanced tactile P300 in response to the delivery of unpredictable relative to predictable shocks during the NPU-threat task (Nelson & Hajcak, 2017b). Together, these results suggest that unpredictability can enhance attentional engagement in response to the actual threat. However, it is still unclear whether increased reactivity to unpredictable threat is associated with individual difference measures of anxiety.

Sex differences in the prevalence of anxiety disorders has been well-documented, with women experiencing higher rates of lifetime diagnoses of almost all anxiety disorders (McLean, Asnaani, Litz, & Hofmann, 2011). A growing number of studies have begun to examine potential mechanisms of the sex difference in anxiety disorders. Recent psychophysiological evidence has also indicated potential sex differences in sensitivity to unpredictable threat, with women demonstrating greater reactivity relative to men (Burani & Nelson, 2020; Grillon, 2008). Notably, these studies have investigated sex differences in anticipatory reactivity, but have not examined reactivity to the actual aversive stimulus.

The present study examined the tactile P300 to the delivery of electric shocks during the NPU-threat task and individual differences in anxiety-related constructs. Specifically, participants completed self-report questionnaires measuring anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty, and neuroticism. We hypothesized that the tactile P300 would be enhanced in response to unpredictable relative to predictable threat. We also hypothesized that greater tactile P300 enhancement to unpredictable threat would be associated with greater anxiety symptoms, intolerance of uncertainty, and neuroticism. Finally, we hypothesized that women would show greater tactile P300 enhancement to unpredictable threat relative to men.

Section snippets

Participants

The sample included 250 undergraduate students from Stony Brook University who participated for course credit. The data were pooled from three separate studies that each used an identical electric shock version of the NPU-threat task (Ferry & Nelson, 2020; Nelson & Hajcak, 2017b). Exclusion criterion was an inability to read or write English. The sample was college-aged (M = 20.15, SD = 2.70), 54.5 % female, and ethnically and racially diverse, including 34.4 % Caucasian, 8.0 % Black, 42.0 %

Clinical characteristics

Table 1 displays correlations between the self-report measures. All self-report measures were correlated with each other, ranging from weak to strong relationships. The IDAS-II subscales were z-scored and averaged to create a composite anxiety symptoms measure. Due to the strong correlations found among responses to these measures, the IUS-12, IUI Part A and B, and the DSIU subscales were z-scored and averaged to create a composite IU measure.

Tactile P300

Fig. 1 displays the tactile P300 waveforms (top) and

Discussion

The present study examined the relationship between the tactile P300 to electric shock administration during the NPU-threat task and individual differences in anxiety symptoms. The tactile P300 was enhanced to unpredictable shocks relative to predictable shocks, replicating previous research demonstrating that temporal unpredictability enhances attentional engagement to threat (Nelson & Hajcak, 2017b). In addition, greater tactile P300 enhancement to unpredictable shocks was associated with

Declaration of Competing Interest

The authors report no declarations of interest.

Acknowledgement

Funding was provided by National Institute of Mental Health (Grant No. K01MH107808).

References (62)

  • C. Grillon et al.

    A review of the modulation of the startle reflex by affective states and its application in psychiatry

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2003)
  • C. Grillon et al.

    Distinct responses to predictable and unpredictable threat in anxiety pathologies: Effect of panic attack

    Biological Psychiatry Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging

    (2017)
  • C.P. McLean et al.

    Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (2011)
  • L.M. McTeague et al.

    Fearful imagery in social phobia: Generalization, comorbidity, and physiological reactivity

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2009)
  • S. Melynyte et al.

    Gender effects on auditory P300: A systematic review

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2018)
  • W.H.R. Miltner et al.

    Event-related brain potentials and affective responses to threat in spider/snake-phobic and non-phobic subjects

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2005)
  • J.S. Moser et al.

    Face processing biases in social anxiety: An electrophysiological study

    Biological Psychology

    (2008)
  • B.D. Nelson et al.

    Does intolerance of uncertainty predict anticipatory startle responses to uncertain threat?

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • J. Polich

    Updating P300: An integrative theory of P3a and P3b

    Clinical Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • O.J. Robinson et al.

    The adaptive threat bias in anxiety: Amygdala-dorsomedial prefrontal cortex coupling and aversive amplification

    NeuroImage

    (2012)
  • S.A. Shankman et al.

    Effects of predictability of shock timing and intensity on aversive responses

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2011)
  • M.A. Thibodeau et al.

    Developing scales measuring disorder-specific intolerance of uncertainty (DSIU): A new perspective on transdiagnostic

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2015)
  • Y. Benjamini et al.

    Controlling the false discovery rate: A practical and powerful approach to multiple testing

    Journal of the Royal Statistical Society

    (1995)
  • K.P. Bennett et al.

    If or when? Uncertainty’s role in anxious anticipation

    Psychophysiology

    (2018)
  • V.C. Blau et al.

    The face-specific N170 component is modulated by emotional facial expression

    Behavioral and Brain Functions

    (2007)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    A multi-process account of startle modulation during affective perception

    Psychophysiology

    (2006)
  • M.M. Bradley et al.

    Cries, screams, and shouts of joy: Affective responses to environmental sounds

    Psychophysiology

    (1994)
  • P.J. De Jong et al.

    Startle reflex modulation in spider phobics: Effects of monaural left and right ear stimulation

    Journal of Psychophysiology

    (1996)
  • R. Dieterich et al.

    Uncertainty is associated with increased selective attention and sustained stimulus processing

    Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • R. Dieterich et al.

    Uncertainty increases neural indices of attention in obsessive-compulsive disorder

    Depression and Anxiety

    (2017)
  • E. Donchin

    Surprise!…Surprise?

    Psychophysiology

    (1981)
  • Cited by (7)

    • Intolerance of uncertainty and neural measures of anticipation and reactivity for affective stimuli

      2023, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      While utilization of fMRI research has been instrumental for advancing the understanding of IU, there has also been emphasis on other neural measures of human physiology to better understand this construct. Recent research on IU modulation relying on Event Related Potentials (ERPs) during uncertain contexts is emerging (Bauer et al., 2020; Carsten et al., 2022; Del Popolo Cristaldi et al., 2021a; Ferry and Nelson, 2021; Malbec et al., 2022). Limited studies have relied on ERPs in order to examined IU and uncertainty in the context of an S1-S2 image viewing paradigm (Gole et al., 2012; Del Popolo Cristaldi et al., 2021b; Cristaldi et al., 2022), where uncertainty can be manipulated by providing varying amounts of information during pre-stimulus cueing (S1) prior to image (S2) presentation (Böcker et al., 2001).

    • A rare scare: The role of intolerance of uncertainty in startle responses and event-related potentials in anticipation of unpredictable threat

      2022, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      This can be tested by directly comparing associations of IU and responses to different aspects of unpredictability. Further comparisons of IU and the neural processing of threat over time—automatic sensory processing and later processing stages that correspond to attentional processes—are limited in relation to NPU threat-based tasks (Ferry and Nelson, 2021), although have been examined in relation to other types of tasks (Bauer et al., 2020; Ferry and Nelson, 2021; MacNamara, 2018; Nelson et al., 2014; Nelson, Kessel, et al., 2016). Accordingly, one major aim of the present study is to identify correlates of IU in the neural dynamics of threat anticipation, thereby broadening our understanding of IU across units of analysis.

    • Unbalanced functional connectivity at rest affects the ERP correlates of affective prediction in high intolerance of uncertainty individuals: A high density EEG investigation

      2022, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Some recent contributions, indeed, proposed that the biases in affective stimuli anticipation (e.g., the above-mentioned inflated threat estimates) typical of high-IU individuals may derive from a disrupted prediction error signaling, which in turns leads to a failure of affective prediction updating (Grupe and Nitschke, 2013). However, experimental studies investigating the modulatory effects of IU on emotional processing as a function of contextual uncertainty have primarily focused on psychophysiological measures (Chin et al., 2016; Lieberman et al., 2016; Morriss, 2019; Morriss et al., 2021a; Morriss et al., 2022b; Morriss et al., 2020; Nelson et al., 2016) rather than neural measures (Bauer et al., 2020; Del Popolo Cristaldi et al., 2021b; Ferry and Nelson, 2021). In a recent high-density electroencephalography (hd-EEG) study (Del Popolo Cristaldi et al., 2021b), we investigated if IU predicted event-related potentials (ERPs) and cortical brain activity along the stages of affective prediction construction, as a function of contextual uncertainty.

    • Neural and Self-report Measures of Sensitivity to Uncertainty as Predictors of COVID-Related Negative Affect

      2022, Psychiatry Research - Neuroimaging
      Citation Excerpt :

      Although U-threat is universally aversive as it diminishes our ability to prepare for future events, research has shown that there are some individuals who are particularly sensitive to U-threat and display maladaptive cognitive and behavioral responses in the face of uncertainty (Carleton, 2012). Individual differences in sensitivity to uncertainty can be measured in several ways, and these methods tend to have moderate convergence (Ferry and Nelson, 2021; Gorka et al., 2014; Shankman et al., 2014; Simmons et al., 2008). One method includes self-report, which asks participants to rate their subjective tolerance of uncertainty.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text