Elsevier

Biological Psychology

Volume 119, September 2016, Pages 42-45
Biological Psychology

Short communication
Theta phase coherence in affective picture processing reveals dysfunctional sensory integration in psychopathic offenders

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2016.06.011Get rights and content

Abstract

Psychopathic offenders are described as emotionally cold, displaying deficits in affective responding. However, research demonstrates that many of the psychopathy-related deficits are moderated by attention, such that under conditions of high attentional and perceptual load psychopathic offenders display deficits in affective responses, but do not in conditions of low load. To date, most studies use measures of defensive reflex (i.e., startle) and conditioning manipulations to examine the impact of load on psychopathy-related processing, but have not examined more direct measures of attention processing. In a sample of adult male offenders, the present study examined time-frequency EEG phase coherence in response to a picture-viewing paradigm that manipulated picture familiarity to assess neural changes in processing based on perceptual demands. Results indicated psychopathy-related differences in the theta response, an index of readiness to perceive and integrate sensory information. These data provide further evidence that psychopathic offenders have disrupted integration of sensory information.

Introduction

Prominent models of psychopathy attribute these offenders’ failures of conscience, antisocial behavior, and insensitivity to affective information to a core emotion deficit. However, substantial evidence indicates that experimental context moderates these emotion deficits. Baskin-Sommers, Curtin, and Newman (2011) propose that this context specificity is associated with an early attention bottleneck that filters multidimensional information in serial, rather than simultaneously, thus hindering the fluid processing of information. Across experimental contexts, psychopathic offenders display normal responses to affective information when it is part of their goal-directed task or embedded in a perceptually simple display, yet their reactions to the same stimuli are deficient when their attention is allocated to an alternative goal or complex aspect of the situation (Baskin-Sommers, Curtin, & Newman, 2013; Decety, Chen, Harenski, & Kiehl, 2013; Meffert, Gazzola, den Boer, Bartels, & Keysers, 2013; Newman, Curtin, Bertsch, & Baskin-Sommers, 2010; Newman and Kosson, 1986, Sadeh and Verona, 2012).

Arguably the strongest evidence for the emotion deficit in psychopathy comes from research examining startle responses during picture viewing. In contrast to non-psychopathic offenders, who display startle potentiation during unpleasant pictures and startle inhibition during pleasant pictures, the startle potentiation to unpleasant pictures appears to be lacking in psychopathic offenders, particularly in offenders high on interpersonal-affective (Factor1) traits (Vaidyanathan, Hall, Patrick, & Bernat, 2011). However, Baskin-Sommers et al. (2013) demonstrated that by manipulating picture familiarity, psychopathic offenders displayed the classic deficit in emotion-modulated startle during novel pictures, but no deficit in emotion-modulated startle during familiar pictures.

Using explicit instruction or condition manipulations, previous work provides strong evidence of dysfunctional attention-emotion processing in psychopathy. It is possible, though, that an attention bottleneck can also affect perceptual and sensory processing (Kastner & Ungerleider, 2000). Previous research shows that the phase coherence of theta, particularly in parietooccipital and primary sensory cortices, represents a neural index of readiness to perceive and integrate sensory inputs, both across and within sensory modalities (Buzsaki, 2005, Lakatos et al., 2009). Moreover, theta phase coherence is modulated by familiarity, possibly indicating greater dynamic coordination in familiar conditions across sensory domains (Miyakoshi, Kanayama, Iidaka, & Ohira, 2010).1 The present study measured theta phase coherence, as an index of readiness to perceive and integrate sensory information, during the picture-viewing paradigm used by Baskin-Sommers et al. (2013). If readiness to perceive and integrate sensory information affects the efficient processing of affective information among offenders with psychopathy, then their theta inter-trial coherence (ITC), much like their defensive startle reactivity, should be impacted by the familiarity manipulation.

Section snippets

Participants

Ninety-nine incarcerated males between the ages of 18 and 45, with an IQ greater than 70, no clinical diagnoses of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, or psychosis, and who were not currently using psychotropic medications were assessed for psychopathy and its related traits with the Psychopathy Checklist-Revised (PCL-R) (Hare, 2003) (see Table S1).

Task

Thirty-six pictures (12 unpleasant, 12 neutral, 12 pleasant) were selected from the International Affective Picture System (Lang, Bradley, & Cuthbert,

Psychopathy

Data were examined in a 2 (familiar, novel) by 3 (pleasant, neutral, unpleasant) General Linear Model (GLM) with PCL-R (z-score) as a continuous factor. Interaction contrasts were used to examine valence (unpleasant vs. pleasant) and affect (unpleasant/pleasant vs. neutral) effects.

Consistent with prior research, there was a significant familiarity main effect, F(1,97) = 4.71, p = 0.032, ηp2 = 0.046, with familiar pictures eliciting greater theta phase coherence than novel pictures. There were no

Discussion

The present study used time-frequency analysis to examine whether sensory processing and integration affects the core affective deficits characteristic of psychopathy. Psychopathic offenders, and offenders high on Factor1, showed enhanced emotion-modulation of theta ITC to familiar, but not novel pictures. In contrast, theta coherence for offenders high on Factor2 was greater for both types of affective novel stimuli. These results suggest that the psychopathy and Factor-related dysregulation

Acknowledgement

This work was supported by grant 5R21DA030876 from NIDA.

References (26)

  • G. Buzsaki

    Theta rhythm of navigation: link between path integration and landmark navigation, episodic and semantic memory

    Hippocampus

    (2005)
  • J. Decety et al.

    An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy

    Frontiers in Human Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • H. Flor et al.

    Aversive Pavlovian conditioning in psychopaths: peripheral and central correlates

    Psychophysiology

    (2002)
  • Cited by (10)

    • Psychopathy and neurodynamic brain functioning: A review of EEG research

      2019, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Some of the findings also fit with theory given that psychopaths are thought to be alert and cognizant of their surroundings but to show a general lack of concern for others. One study showed disrupted short- and long-range neural communication (Tillem et al., 2018) suggesting less efficient communication although this research is in its infancy and further investigation is needed on spectra data to draw firm conclusions. A number of studies have attempted to examine location or the topography of neural firing through site analyses.

    • Psychopathy is associated with shifts in the organization of neural networks in a large incarcerated male sample

      2019, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      For example, during attention tasks (e.g., Flanker, attention blink) individuals higher in psychopathy reliably exhibit superior performance when asked to attend to goal-relevant information while ignoring goal-irrelevant distractors (Hiatt et al., 2004; Hoppenbrouwers et al., 2015; Wolf et al., 2012; Zeier et al., 2009). As another example, during affective processing tasks in which affective information is goal-relevant, individuals higher in psychopathy show intact, or even enhanced, affective responses (Baskin-Sommers et al., 2011a, 2013b; Decety et al., 2013a; Larson et al., 2013; Newman et al., 2010; Schultz et al., 2016; Tillem and Baskin-Sommers, 2018; Tillem et al., 2016), but, when affective information is goal-irrelevant, they exhibit blunted affective responses (Baskin-Sommers et al., 2011a, 2013b; Larson et al., 2013; Newman et al., 2010). Across all of these experimental paradigms, psychopathic individuals show an enhanced ability to respond to goal-relevant information while ignoring distracting, goal-irrelevant stimuli (e.g., quickly and accurately responding to targets during a flanker-type task, while effectively ignoring goal-irrelevant distractors; Zeier et al., 2009).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text