Research article
Self-organization of grid fields under supervision of place cells in a neuron model with associative plasticity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bica.2015.06.006Get rights and content

Abstract

The grid cells (GCs) of the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) and place cells (PCs) of the hippocampus are assumed to be the key elements of the brain network for the metric representation of space. Existing theoretical models of GC network rely on specific hypotheses about the network connectivity patterns. How these patterns could be formed during the network development is not fully understood. It was previously suggested, within the feedforward network models, that activity of PCs could provide the basis for development of GC-like activity patterns. Supporting this hypothesis is the finding that PC activity remains spatially stable after disruption of the GC firing patterns and that the grid fields almost disappear when hippocampal cells are deactivated. Here a new theoretical model of this type is proposed, allowing for grid fields formation due to synaptic plasticity in synapses connecting PCs in hippocampus with neurons in MEC. Learning of the hexagonally symmetric fields in this model occurs due to complex action of several simple biologically motivated synaptic plasticity rules. These rules include associative synaptic plasticity rules similar to BCM rule, and homeostatic plasticity rules that constrain synaptic weights. In contrast to previously described models, a short-term navigational experience in a novel environment is sufficient for the network to learn GC activity patterns. We suggest that learning on the basis of simple and biologically plausible associative synaptic plasticity rules could contribute to the formation of grid fields in early development and to maintenance of normal GCs activity patterns in the familiar environments.

Introduction

The grid cells (GCs) in the medial entorhinal cortex (MEC) of mammals are the neurons whose firing activity during animal navigation is concentrated near the centers of hexagonally symmetric grid covering the environment (Hafting et al., 2005, Fyhn et al., 2007, Moser et al., 2008, Moser et al., 2015, Stensola et al., 2012). The network of GCs is considered to be a component of the brain system for metric representation of space and navigation (Hartley et al., 2014, Moser et al., 2008, Moser et al., 2015). Patterns of activity, connectivity and development of GC networks have been extensively investigated, and a number of theoretical models of GC network was developed during last decade (see Moser et al., 2014, Zilli, 2012 for review and Bush and Burgess, 2014, Castro and Aguiar, 2014, Hasselmo and Shay, 2014, Pilly and Grossberg, 2013, Widloski and Fiete, 2014 for recent models). These models differ significantly in the way they represent current position of the animal. For example, models based on continuous attractor networks (CAN) (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005, Fuhs and Touretzky, 2006, McNaughton et al., 2006, O’Keefe and Burgess, 2005), interference of slow oscillations (Blair et al., 2007, Burgess et al., 2007, Gaussier et al., 2007, Giocomo et al., 2007) and feedforward networks (Castro and Aguiar, 2014, Kropff and Treves, 2008, Pilly and Grossberg, 2013) exist. Depending on the nature of input signals these models could be divided into path-integrating models and models that utilize location-specific combinations of input signals. Currently, none of the existing GC network models could be considered as sufficiently complete and supported by the experimental data.

Most of the GC network models could be assigned to the path-integrating type. They imply the emergence of a network, controlled by the signals about animal’s speed and direction of movement, in order to provide integration of movements along the path (Moser et al., 2014, Zilli, 2012). Models of this type have a number of restrictions. For example, path integrating models use only a small part of sensory input signals from those which are received by EC and could be the source of navigational information. Other set of problems arise because path integration process in these models is assumed to be continuous, and gradually accumulating integration errors should constantly be adjusted by some additional mechanism (Cheung et al., 2012, Hardcastle et al., 2015).

Most of the existing models of path-integrating type rely on relatively complex predefined patterns of network connectivity and do not address the problem of these patterns development. For example, many path-integrating models, which use CAN for animal position representation (Conklin and Eliasmith, 2005, Fuhs and Touretzky, 2006, McNaughton et al., 2006, O’Keefe and Burgess, 2005), are based on hypotheses that (1) two-dimensional neuronal network with toroidal topology of synaptic connections exists in the entorhinal cortex, (2) strength and symmetry of these connections are high enough for a stable localized bump or a periodic set of bumps of activity to exist in the network, and (3) additional groups of neurons exist with activity, modulated by speed and direction of animal movement, and with asymmetric connectivity within attractor network layer. As a result, the bump of activity in 2D layer of neurons could move in a coordinated manner with animal movement. Several possible scenarios of development of path integrating CAN were proposed (Hahnloser, 2003, McNaughton et al., 2006, Stringer et al., 2002, Widloski and Fiete, 2014). The problem of development of most of connectivity patterns, assumed by hypotheses (1)–(3), was directly addressed in a recent model of Widloski and Fiete (2014). In their work, formation of path-integrating 2D network of GCs under control of both location-specific and velocity-modulated input signals was demonstrated. This network was obtained as a result of self-organization process driven by spike-time dependent plasticity (STDP) in the lateral synaptic connections in the network of excitatory and inhibitory neurons.

Another type of the GC models suggests that the feedforward network connecting neurons with spatially modulated patterns of activity, such as hippocampal place cells (PCs) or MEC/parasubicular stripe cells, to GCs is formed due to associative synaptic plasticity. (Castro and Aguiar, 2014, Kropff and Treves, 2008, Pilly and Grossberg, 2013, Si et al., 2012). In several models of this type, formation of grid fields in a given environment is associated with formation of synaptic connections to GCs selectively from those PCs whose firing fields are located in a nodes of hexagonal lattice (Castro and Aguiar, 2014, Kropff and Treves, 2008). This connectivity pattern is supported by the finding that PC activity remains spatially stable after the disruption of GC firing patterns (Bush et al., 2014, Hales et al., 2014, Koenig et al., 2011) and that grid patterns almost disappear when hippocampal cells are deactivated (Bonnevie et al., 2013). Development of the early PCs before GCs formation also supports the role of PCs as spatial information providers to GCs (Langston et al., 2010, Wills et al., 2010).

The limitation of models based on PC–GC feedforward network self-organization (Castro and Aguiar, 2014, Kropff and Treves, 2008) is that they do not explain explicitly how normal pattern of GC activity could persist during animal’s navigation in darkness (Hafting et al., 2005). Other GC network property which seems to be difficult to explain within PC–GC feedforward network hypothesis is the fact that in a novel environment relative phases of grid fields in the pairs of GCs do not change, while place fields rebuilt completely and their relative phases in PC pairs before and after remapping are not correlated (Fyhn et al., 2007). As it was described in the work of Kropff and Treves (Kropff & Treves, 2008), alignment of grid fields orientations in the network and correlated remapping of grid fields in different environments could be, in principle, achieved if GCs activity before grid fields formation is modulated by head direction cells and if the strength of horizontal excitatory synaptic connections, which form between pairs of GCs in 2D layer of GCs, is modulated by the degree of similarity of their preferred head directions and by their relative distance in 2D layer. In the work of Si et al. (2012) it was shown that coordinated remapping of grid fields between two environments indeed can be observed in the model if the sets of presynaptic PCs active in these environments are weakly overlapping. Modular organization of GC networks, and the fact that formation of new grid fields in a novel environment usually do not require prolonged sensory experience in this environment, are the other issues that require explanation within the framework of feedforward network models (Barry et al., 2007, Barry et al., 2012, Stensola et al., 2012).

Models based on PC–GC feedforward network self-organization rely on specific plasticity rules and differ by their characteristics. For example, the model of Kropff and Treves (Kropff and Treves, 2008, Si et al., 2012) assumes that the mechanism of GCs firing rate adaptation besides Hebbian plasticity rules in PC–GC synapses is important for grid fields development. The model of Castro and Aguiar (Castro & Aguiar, 2014) assumes the existence of a specific complex rule linking synaptic states and animal’s position. At the same time, it is well-known that the patterns with hexagonal symmetry are one of the most ubiquitous types of patterns that emerge as a result of symmetry-breaking bifurcations in many physical systems (Golubitsky, Stewart, & Schaeffer, 1988), in particular in the models of self-organization based on Hebbian plasticity in neural networks described by the neural fields equations (Bressloff et al., 2001, Ermentrout and Cowan, 1979). Thus, it is interesting to explore other possible mechanisms of grid fields development, with a more general types of plasticity rules involved.

The goal of this work is to describe a class of models in which the grid fields are formed as a result of plasticity of PC–GC synapses or synapses from sensory neurons to GC, and to demonstrate a number of possible grid fields formation scenarios in these models.

The main difference of the model proposed in this paper from the previously proposed models is that it uses only simplest rules of synaptic plasticity, similar to those supported by experimental data, and does not require assumptions about the firing rate adaptation.

The most important assumption made in this work is that dependence of associative plasticity rate of PC–GC synapses on the frequency of presynaptic action potentials has a minimum within a typical range of PCs activity during animal’s exploration of environment. This assumption is supported by a number of experimental works demonstrating that transition from long term depression (LTD) at low pre- and postsynaptic firing rates to long term potentiation (LTP) at high rates could occur in hippocampal (Artola and Singer, 1993, Bear and Malenka, 1994, Dudek and Bear, 1992, Mayford et al., 1995, Wang and Wagner, 1999) and entorhinal cortex synapses (Deng and Lei, 2007, Solger et al., 2004, Zhou et al., 2005). The second assumption is that some components of associative or homeostatic synaptic plasticity depend nonlinearly on synaptic weights, and that the curvature of this dependence is positive. This assumption seem to be physiologically relevant since it simply results, for example, from lower-bounding of excitatory synapses weights.

Learning of grid fields in the proposed model could be very fast and does not require a lot of exploratory experience in a novel environment. In addition, the proposed model is compatible with CAN models of GC network and could serve as a basis for the path integration error correction, and for the self-organization of activity bump position control in them.

Section snippets

Description of the model

In this work we develop a hypothesis that grid cells recognize sets of sensory signals, associated with location of an animal in certain places of environment, with the help of clusters of synapses from hippocampal place cells to grid cell or from neurons of different sensory systems to grid cell. Clusters are composed of synapses with correlated presynaptic activity and form as a result of Hebbian associative plasticity. In addition, under the action of both associative synaptic plasticity and

Results

To confirm previous analysis we have conducted a series of computational experiments. First, we have found that formation of hexagonally symmetric fields can be observed for a broad ranges of the second order terms coefficients η2+ and ε2 in Eq. (8). Typical results of computational experiments are shown in Fig. 1.

Linear neuron with 961 synapses was modeled. Each synapse received spatially modulated input from a PC with Gaussian-shaped firing field centered on a vertex of 31 × 31 hexagonal

Discussion

In this work we have described formation of grid-like spatially selective sensory fields in the group of models of EC neuron having linear activation function and receiving spatially modulated input from place cells or from neurons of different sensory systems.

We have shown that learning of hexagonally symmetric fields in these models is a result of complex action of several simple biologically motivated synaptic plasticity rules. These rules include rules for associative synaptic plasticity

Conclusions

  • 1.

    Learning of hexagonally symmetric fields demonstrated in the model of EC neuron with linear activation function. The neuron received spatially modulated input from place cells, or cells from sensory systems. Learning rules used in the model have a general form and are biologically motivated.

  • 2.

    From the perspective of pattern formation theory, associative synaptic plasticity, linear with respect to synaptic weights, is required for the selection of patterns composed of waves with similar critical

Acknowledgments

The author would like to thank Alexei Samsonovich for the suggestion of the idea of principal component analysis of place cells activity patterns as a possible mechanism of grid fields formation, that lay down in the basis of this work. This work was supported by the Ukrainian government.

References (58)

  • E.L. Bienenstock et al.

    Theory for the development of neuron selectivity: Orientation specificity and binocular interaction in visual cortex

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (1982)
  • G.Q. Bi et al.

    Synaptic modifications in cultured hippocampal neurons: Dependence on spike timing, synaptic strength, and postsynaptic cell type

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (1998)
  • H.T. Blair et al.

    Scale-invariant memory representations emerge from MOIR interference between grid fields that produce theta oscillations: A computational model

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (2007)
  • T. Bonnevie et al.

    Grid cells require excitatory drive from the hippocampus

    Nature Neurosciences

    (2013)
  • P.C. Bressloff

    Spontaneous symmetry breaking in self-organizing neural fields

    Biological Cybernetics

    (2005)
  • P.C. Bressloff et al.

    Geometric visual hallucinations, Euclidean symmetry and the functional architecture of striate cortex

    Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London, B, Biological Sciences

    (2001)
  • N. Burgess et al.

    An oscillatory interference model of grid cell firing

    Hippocampus

    (2007)
  • D. Bush et al.

    A hybrid oscillatory interference/continuous attractor network model of grid cell firing

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (2014)
  • D. Bush et al.

    Reconciling the STDP and BCM models of synaptic plasticity in a spiking recurrent neural network

    Neural Computation

    (2010)
  • L. Castro et al.

    A feedforward model for the formation of a grid field where spatial information is provided solely from place cells

    Biological Cybernetics

    (2014)
  • A. Cheung et al.

    Maintaining a cognitive map in darkness: The need to fuse boundary knowledge with path integration

    PLoS Computational Biology

    (2012)
  • J. Conklin et al.

    A controlled attractor network model of path integration in the rat

    Journal of Computational Neurosciences

    (2005)
  • L.N. Cooper et al.

    The BCM theory of synapse modification at 30: Interaction of theory with experiment

    Nature Review Neurosciences

    (2012)
  • J. Csicsvari et al.

    Sharp wave/ripple network oscillations and learning-associated hippocampal maps

    Philosophical Transactions of Royal Society of London, B, Biological Sciences

    (2014)
  • P.Y. Deng et al.

    Long-term depression in identified stellate neurons of juvenile rat entorhinal cortex

    Journal of Neurophysiology

    (2007)
  • S.M. Dudek et al.

    Homosynaptic long-term depression in area CA1 of hippocampus and effects of N-methyl-D-aspartate receptor blockade

    Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA

    (1992)
  • B. Ermentrout

    Stripes or spots? Nonlinear effects in bifurcation of reaction–diffusion equations on the square

    Proceedings of the Royal Society of London – Series A

    (1991)
  • G.B. Ermentrout et al.

    A mathematical theory of visual hallucination patterns

    Biological Cybernetics

    (1979)
  • M.C. Fuhs et al.

    A spin glass model of path integration in rat medial entorhinal cortex

    Journal of Neurosciences

    (2006)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text