Elsevier

Behavioural Brain Research

Volume 332, 14 August 2017, Pages 136-144
Behavioural Brain Research

The neural correlates of reciprocity are sensitive to prior experience of reciprocity

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2017.05.030Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Non-reciprocity was associated with precentral gyrus and culmen activation.

  • Altruism was associated with lingual gyrus activation.

  • Reciprocity was associated with activity in distinct neural networks.

Abstract

Reciprocity is central to human relationships and is strongly influenced by multiple factors including the nature of social exchanges and their attendant emotional reactions. Despite recent advances in the field, the neural processes involved in this modulation of reciprocal behavior by ongoing social interaction are poorly understood. We hypothesized that activity within a discrete set of neural networks including a putative moral cognitive neural network is associated with reciprocity behavior. Nineteen healthy adults underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging scanning while playing the trustee role in the Trust Game. Personality traits and moral development were assessed. Independent component analysis was used to identify task-related functional brain networks and assess their relationship to behavior. The saliency network (insula and anterior cingulate) was positively correlated with reciprocity behavior. A consistent array of brain regions supports the engagement of emotional, self-referential and planning processes during social reciprocity behavior.

Introduction

Social reciprocity, defined as behavioral response to a positive action with another positive action, is central to an individual’s development of healthy interpersonal relationships, learning of social norms and normative ethics, and integration into society [1]. Reciprocity and other prosocial behaviors, such as altruism, fairness, and cooperation, are adaptive behaviors common to many cultures and religious traditions. Human subjects engage in prosocial behaviors even at their own expense [2], and thus reciprocity can overlap with altruistic behavior. However, reciprocity and other prosocial behaviors are multi-determined and dissociable products of complex and dynamic transactional processes [3]. For instance, reciprocity can be seen as self-serving, whether by promoting the benefit of future reciprocity, increasing one’s social status or reputation, resolving the conflict of seeing somebody else suffer, enabling redemption or release from guilt, or even by increasing reproductive population fitness [2], [4], [5], [6], [7].

The pervasive human distinction between “us” and “others” is traditionally based on kinships but tends to be determined by social proximity and contact. Personal interaction with others makes decision making less utilitarian and more emotionally driven [8]. For example, when research subjects believe they are interacting with other humans (rather than computers) neuroimaging has shown greater engagement of the ventral striatum and ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPFC), brain regions that are both associated with reward processing [9], [10]. However, the neural processes underlying the modulating effects of social interaction, either positive or negative, on basic reciprocal behavior are largely unknown.

Monetary exchange paradigms have yielded valuable insight into the neurobiology of cooperation [11], trust [12], [13], and agency [14]. In the present study, we sought to further characterize the neural mechanisms underlying individual variation in reciprocity behavior during a monetary decision making task, the Trust Game. Variations in task conditions allowed to assess the behavioral and neural processing correlates of reciprocating a gift before and after a social exchange in which a gift to another was or was not reciprocated. We hypothesized that reciprocity correlates with the engagement of neural processing networks including a putative moral cognitive neural network composed of the frontal pole, anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), superior temporal cortex (STS), precuneus, and posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) [15], [16], [17].

Section snippets

Participants

We recruited 30 participants aged 18–30 years, 12 men and 18 women. The racially and ethnically diverse sample consisted of 15 Caucasians, 13 Hispanics, and 2 African Americans. Twenty-four participants were college graduate or undergraduate students, five were employed, and one participant was unemployed. Eleven participants were excluded from the brain imaging analysis due to head motion artifact (n = 1) or for not varying their behavioral response (always reciprocating), which precluded

Behavioral outcomes

In the Baseline condition, the 19 participants included in the fMRI analysis reciprocated (Gave $1.5X) 61.9% of the time. During the EC condition, participants reciprocated after their giving was reciprocated 78.5% of the time, but only 53.5% when their giving was not reciprocated. The binary logistic model showed a significant effect of Reciprocity behavior in the Baseline condition (p < 0.001). See Table 2.

The Personal Interest score (DIT-2) correlated positively with reciprocity behavior in

Discussion

The tendency to reciprocate is diminished by the unfair act of others not reciprocating in a prior exchange, though some individuals persist in reciprocating following a perceived violation of reciprocity by others. This study sought to identify the neural correlates of such behavior after fair versus unfair exchanges. We have identified a set of neural processing networks (DMN, SAL, CEN, and LIM) functionally associated with reciprocity behavior. Specifically, we report greater SAL network

Conclusions

Our findings of an association of distinct neural networks (DMN, SAL, CEN and LIM) with reciprocity behavior during the Trust Game add to a growing body of evidence linking involvement of a recurrent set of brain regions, i.e precuneus, superior temporal cortex, insula, medial prefrontal cortex and anterior cingulate, with complex moral emotions and behavior. Even though it may be premature to name these regions a moral network, their consistent involvement in moral-related experiments supports

Funding

This work was supported by the Ethics and Community Program from the Arsht Foundation.

Acknowledgments

Editing assistance was provided by the Office of Grants and Scientific Publications, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences.

References (63)

  • C.R. Genovese et al.

    Thresholding of statistical maps in functional neuroimaging using the false discovery rate

    Neuroimage

    (2002)
  • T.D. Wager et al.

    Increased sensitivity in neuroimaging analyses using robust regression

    Neuroimage

    (2005)
  • E. Fehr et al.

    Social neuroeconomics: the neural circuitry of social preferences

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2007)
  • W.G. Ekins et al.

    You cannot gamble on others: dissociable systems forstrategic uncertainty and risk in the brain

    J. Econ. Behav. Organ.

    (2013)
  • D. Robertson et al.

    The neural processing of moral sensitivity to issues of justice and care

    Neuropsychologia

    (2007)
  • F. Van Overwalle et al.

    Understanding others' actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: a meta-analysis

    Neuroimage

    (2009)
  • A. Strobel et al.

    Beyond revenge: neural and genetic bases of altruistic punishment

    Neuroimage

    (2011)
  • T. Baumgartner et al.

    The neural circuitry of a broken promise

    Neuron

    (2009)
  • S.S. Ho et al.

    Neurocircuits underlying cognition-emotion interaction in a social decision making context

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • N.D. Johnson et al.

    Trust games: a meta-analysis

    J. Econ. Psychol.

    (2011)
  • J.K. Rilling et al.

    Effects of intranasal oxytocin and vasopressin on cooperative behavior and associated brain activity in men

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2012)
  • R. Axelrod et al.

    The evolution of cooperation

    Science

    (1981)
  • S.F. Brosnan et al.

    The interplay of cognition and cooperation

    Philos. Trans. R Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci.

    (2010)
  • B.J. Seelig et al.

    Normal and pathological altruism

    J. Am. Psychoanal. Assoc.

    (2010)
  • A. Lotem et al.

    From reciprocity to unconditional altruism through signalling benefits

    Proc. Biol. Sci.

    (2003)
  • J.D. Greene et al.

    An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment

    Science

    (2001)
  • J. Katsyri et al.

    The opponent matters: elevated FMRI reward responses to winning against a human versus a computer opponent during interactive video game playing

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2013)
  • K. McCabe et al.

    A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2001)
  • F. Krueger et al.

    Neural correlates of trust

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2007)
  • B. King-Casas et al.

    Getting to know you: reputation and trust in a two-person economic exchange

    Science

    (2005)
  • D. Tomlin et al.

    Agent-specific responses in the cingulate cortex during economic exchanges

    Science

    (2006)
  • View full text