Research report
Experience with the high-intensity sweetener saccharin impairs glucose homeostasis and GLP-1 release in rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2012.04.024Get rights and content

Abstract

Previous work from our lab has demonstrated that experience with high-intensity sweeteners in rats leads to increased food intake, body weight gain and adiposity, along with diminished caloric compensation and decreased thermic effect of food. These changes may occur as a result of interfering with learned relations between the sweet taste of food and the caloric or nutritive consequences of consuming those foods. The present experiments determined whether experience with the high-intensity sweetener saccharin versus the caloric sweetener glucose affected blood glucose homeostasis. The results demonstrated that during oral glucose tolerance tests, blood glucose levels were more elevated in animals that had previously consumed the saccharin-sweetened supplements. In contrast, during glucose tolerance tests when a glucose solution was delivered directly into the stomach, no differences in blood glucose levels between the groups were observed. Differences in oral glucose tolerance responses were not accompanied by differences in insulin release; insulin release was similar in animals previously exposed to saccharin and those previously exposed to glucose. However, release of GLP-1 in response to an oral glucose tolerance test, but not to glucose tolerance tests delivered by gavage, was significantly lower in saccharin-exposed animals compared to glucose-exposed animals. Differences in both blood glucose and GLP-1 release in saccharin animals were rapid and transient, and suggest that one mechanism by which exposure to high-intensity sweeteners that interfere with a predictive relation between sweet tastes and calories may impair energy balance is by suppressing GLP-1 release, which could alter glucose homeostasis and reduce satiety.

Highlights

► Rats given dietary supplements sweetened with saccharin gained extra body weight. ► Saccharin-exposed animals were hyperglycemic during oral glucose tolerance tests. ► Hyperglycemia in saccharin-exposed animals was not due to changes in insulin release. ► Reduced release of GLP-1 was observed in saccharin-exposed animals. ► Decreased release of GLP-1 may explain both hyperglycemia and increased food intake.

Introduction

Previous data from our lab, and others, indicates that sweet and fatty orosensory stimuli that do not reliably predict the post-ingestive energetic consequences of consumption can impair the ability of rats to regulate both short-term and longer-term food intake and body weight. For example, rats given dietary supplements with the high-intensity sweetener saccharin, which provides a strong sweet taste, but does not deliver calories, exhibit poorer caloric compensation for novel sweet-tasting pre-meals in short-term intake tests [1], [2]. Further, over the long-term, consumption of a saccharin-sweetened yogurt supplements resulted in increased energy intake, increased body weight gain and increased adiposity relative to consumption of the same supplements sweetened with glucose [3], [4], [5].

We have suggested that mechanisms that underlie the dysregulation of energy balance when consuming high-intensity sweeteners may involve reducing the validity of a predictive relationship between sweet taste and the delivery of energy or calories. Based on fundamental principles of Pavlovian conditioning, presentation of a cue without its anticipated consequence will weaken the ability of the cue to evoke the conditioned responses [6], [7], [8]. In the case of sweet tastes, high-intensity sweeteners provide a strong sweet cue, but without the delivery of an anticipated energetic or caloric outcome. This would be expected to weaken the validity of the sweet taste  calorie relationship. To the extent that the ability to anticipate the caloric consequences of intake contributes to energy, and ultimately body weight regulation, animals may both overeat and gain excess body weight when sweet tastes that do provide calories are encountered. Consistent with this hypothesis, we have recently demonstrated that consumption of non-caloric sweeteners selectively impairs the ability or rats to regulate intake and weight gain when they are maintained on a sweetened high-calorie diet [9]. We have recently extended the generality of this account by showing that energy balance can also be disrupted by exposure to fatty-tasting foods manufactured with fat substitutes that mimic the sensory properties of fat, but without calories [10]. Similar to effects seen with sweet tastes, positive energy balance is the result of the selective overconsumption fatty foods that do deliver energy.

While food intake, weight gain and adiposity are increased by exposure to such substitutes, the specific physiological mechanisms that contribute to overconsumption and impaired energy balance have not been specified. One potential mechanism is interference with physiological responses that anticipate the arrival of energy and nutrients upon consumption of foods. The role of experience with environmental cues in modulating such physiological responses have been the focus of study since the foundational work of Pavlov [11] and these “cephalic phase” responses have been documented in both pre-clinical rodent studies, as well as clinical studies in humans. Cephalic phase responses, such as the release of insulin, are thought to reflect fractional components of physiological responses recruited to efficiently metabolize ingested foods [11], [12], [13], [14], [15]. They may also serve to modulate meal size by generating satiety signals. The strength of such responses should be determined, at least in part, by the validity of the predictive relationship between taste cues and their consequences. Accordingly, the magnitude of cephalic phase responses should be weakened to the extent that exposure to taste cues that are not followed by energetic outcomes weakens the validity of the normal sweet taste  calorie predictive relationship.

Previous studies show that consuming noncaloric sweeteners can evoke cephalic phase responses in rats (e.g. [14], [16], [17], [18], [19]). However, whether the magnitude of these responses is reduced following extended experience with these sweeteners is presently unknown. Thus, the goal of the present studies was to determine whether experience with sweet tastes that do not predict energetic outcomes alters cephalic phase responses as indexed by the ability of rats to modulate blood glucose levels in response to sweet-tasting, high calorie foods. The hypothesis was that compared to animals given diets in which sweet taste always predicted the delivery of increased calories and glucose, animals given diets in which sweet taste did not predict calories (using saccharin) would show a hyperglycemic response to a sweet-tasting caloric load. In contrast, glycemic responses to sweet-tasting, caloric diets which bypass the oral cavity, thereby precluding cephalic phases reflexes evoked by oral taste, were expected to be similar between groups. In a series of experiments, we examined the effects of exposure to yogurt diet supplements (Experiments 1–4, 6) and solutions (Experiment 5) on glycemic responses to a novel, sweet-tasting test meal (Experiment 1), or a glucose solution (Experiments 2–6) to determine whether experience consuming sweet tastes that were not associated with calories or sugar resulted in alterations in blood glucose homeostasis. In addition, we examined whether levels of the peptide hormones insulin and GLP-1 were altered following experience with saccharin-sweetened diet supplements compared to glucose-sweetened diet supplements since release of both insulin and GLP-1 are critical in regulation of glucose homeostasis, and have been implicated in satiety and modulation of meal size (e.g. [20], [21], [22], [23], [24], [25], [26], [27], [28], [29], [30], [31], [32]).

Section snippets

Subjects

All animals were adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (Harlan, Indianapolis, IN). Animals were housed individually in the laboratory and given ad lib. access to water and laboratory chow (Harlan 2018) for at least one week prior to being assigned to groups matched on body weight.

Diets and dietary supplements

In Experiments 1–4, animals were given access to 30 g yogurt (Dannon lowfat yogurt; ∼0.6 kcal/g) daily as a dietary supplement. Yogurt supplements were provided in plain, unsweetened form 3 days per week and in sweetened form

Body weight

Starting body weight did not differ between the two groups (means ± SEM = 390.0 ± 3.5 and 391.8 ± 3.6 for glucose and saccharin groups, respectively). Body weight gain was significantly affected by both the day of yogurt exposure and the Sweetener group (Main effect of Sweetener, F 1, 29 = 7.38, p = 0.011; Main effect of Day, F 12, 348 = 343.8, p < 0.0000001; Sweetener × Day interaction, F 12, 348 = 4.47, p = 0.000001; Fig. 1). Animals given the saccharin-sweetened yogurt gained significantly more weight than

Discussion

Previous work from our lab indicated that following exposure to dietary supplements of saccharin-sweetened yogurt, rats showed both impaired ability to compensate for calories and a diminished thermic effect of food following consumption of a novel, sweet-tasting test meal of thickened Chocolate Ensure Plus [2]. Therefore, the goal of Experiment 1 was to determine whether exposure to saccharin-sweetened yogurt altered glycemic responses to this novel test meal compared to exposure to

Conclusion

These studies support a role for interfering with learned relations between sweet tastes and calories in the regulation of not only body weight, but glucose homeostasis. Animals given experience with sweet tastes that are not associated with calories show hyperglycemia and reduced secretion of GLP-1 during subsequent oral glucose tolerance tests, while secretion of insulin appears to be unchanged. The decrease in GLP-1 may play a direct role in the dysregulation of food intake and body weight.

Role of funding source

The funding source had no role in the study design, collection, analysis, interpretation of the results or the decision to submit the article for publication.

Acknowledgments

We thank Melissa McCurley, David Katz, Ethan Flint, Sean Ogden and Natalie Rappaport for technical assistance with these experiments. Supported by R01DK076078, R01DK076078-03S1 and P01HD052112.

References (81)

  • K.L. Teff

    How neural mediation of anticipatory and compensatory insulin release helps us tolerate food

    Physiology & Behavior

    (2011)
  • M.E. Doyle et al.

    Mechanisms of action of glucagon-like peptide 1 in the pancreas

    Pharmacology & Therapeutics

    (2007)
  • E. Naslund et al.

    Glucagon-like peptide 1 increases the period of postprandial satiety and slows gastric emptying in obese men

    The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (1998)
  • A. Redondo et al.

    Cell signalling of the GLP-1 action in rat liver

    Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology

    (2003)
  • J.M. Wishart et al.

    Relation between gastric emptying of glucose and plasma concentrations of glucagon-like peptide-1

    Peptides

    (1998)
  • T. Wu et al.

    Effects of different sweet preloads on incretin hormone secretion, gastric emptying, and postprandial glycemia in healthy humans

    The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition

    (2012)
  • A.A. Young et al.

    Dose-responses for the slowing of gastric emptying in a rodent model by glucagon-like peptide (7-36) NH2, amylin, cholecystokinin, and other possible regulators of nutrient uptake

    Metabolism

    (1996)
  • K.J. Rudenga et al.

    Amygdala response to sucrose consumption is inversely related to artificial sweetener use

    Appetite

    (2012)
  • B. Verges et al.

    Beyond glucose lowering: glucagon-like peptide-1 receptor agonists, body weight and the cardiovascular system

    Diabetes & Metabolism

    (2011)
  • T.L. Davidson et al.

    A Pavlovian approach to the problem of obesity

    International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders

    (2004)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    A role for sweet taste: calorie predictive relations in energy regulation by rats

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2008)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    General and persistent effects of high-intensity sweeteners on body weight gain and caloric compensation in rats

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2009)
  • R.A. Rescorla et al.

    A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement

  • K. Urushihara et al.

    Stimulus competition between a discrete cue and a training contex: cue competition does not result from the division of a limited resource

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes

    (2009)
  • A.R. Wagner

    Stimulus validity and stimulus selection in associative learning

  • T.L. Davidson et al.

    Intake of high-intensity sweeteners alters the ability of sweet taste to signal caloric consequences: implications for the learned control of energy and body weight regulation

    The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Hove)

    (2011)
  • S.E. Swithers et al.

    Fat substitutes promote weight gain in rats consuming high-fat diets

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • I.P. Pavlov

    Conditioned reflexes

    (1927)
  • S.C. Woods

    The eating paradox: how we tolerate food

    Psychological Review

    (1991)
  • H.R. Berthoud et al.

    Cephalic phase, reflex insulin secretion. Neuroanatomical and physiological characterization

    Diabetologia

    (1981)
  • J. Louis-Sylvestre

    Relationship between two stages of prandial insulin release in rats

    American Journal of Physiology

    (1978)
  • K. Tonosaki et al.

    Relationships between insulin release and taste

    Biomedical Research

    (2007)
  • J.G. Barrera et al.

    Hyperphagia and increased fat accumulation in two models of chronic CNS glucagon-like peptide-1 loss of function

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • J.G. Barrera et al.

    GLP-1 and energy balance: an integrated model of short-term and long-term control

    Nature Reviews Endocrinology

    (2011)
  • A.M. Dossat et al.

    Glucagon-like peptide 1 receptors in nucleus accumbens affect food intake

    Journal of Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • D.J. Drucker

    Minireview: the glucagon-like peptides

    Endocrinology

    (2001)
  • A. Flint et al.

    Glucagon-like peptide 1 promotes satiety and suppresses energy intake in humans

    Journal of Clinical Investigation

    (1998)
  • H.J. Grill et al.

    The nucleus tractus solitarius: a portal for visceral afferent signal processing, energy status assessment and integration of their combined effects on food intake

    International Journal of Obesity (London)

    (2009)
  • C. Knauf et al.

    Brain glucagon-like peptide 1 signaling controls the onset of high-fat diet-induced insulin resistance and reduces energy expenditure

    Endocrinology

    (2008)
  • M.D. Turton et al.

    A role for glucagon-like peptide-1 in the central regulation of feeding

    Nature

    (1996)
  • Cited by (87)

    • Food obesogens as emerging metabolic disruptors; A toxicological insight

      2022, Journal of Steroid Biochemistry and Molecular Biology
    • Association of beverage consumption types with weight, height, and body mass index in grade 3 children in northern Taiwan: A cross-sectional study

      2021, Nutrition
      Citation Excerpt :

      Additionally, artificial sweeteners may not have the same inhibitory effect on food intake as regular caloric sweeteners, which inhibit motilin secretion and antral motility and increase cholecystokinin secretion [34]. Moreover, artificial sweeteners may decrease the release of glucagon-like peptide-1, which can regulate food intake and blood sugar [35]. Thus, products containing artificial sweeteners may not be as ineffectual to health as originally thought, and the overall reduction of sweetened beverages should be emphasized.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text