Elsevier

Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Volume 83, July–August 2019, Pages 114-120
Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics

Refining memory assessment of elderly people with cognitive impairment: Insights from the short-term memory binding test

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.archger.2019.03.025Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Short-term memory binding (STMB) can inform about the predementia stages of AD.

  • Tailoring the test difficulty to the disease severity helps trace the AD continuum.

  • STMB and hippocampal volume identify MCI patients who may be on the AD trajectory.

Abstract

Alzheimer's disease (AD) affects temporary memory for bound features more remarkably than for individual features. Such selective impairments manifest from presymptomatic through dementia stages via titration procedures. A recent study suggested that without titration and with high memory load the binding selectivity may disappear in people at risk of AD such as those with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI). We compared data from two studies on temporary binding which assessed people with MCI and controls using different memory loads (2 or 3 items). Selective binding impairments were found in MCI, but relative to controls, such selectivity was contingent upon memory load (i.e., present with 2 items). Further analysis with MCI people who tested positive to neuroimaging biomarkers (i.e., hippocampal atrophy) confirmed that this specific binding impairments are a feature of prodromal AD. The temporary binding task has been recently suggested by consensus papers as a potential screening tool for AD. The results presented here inform on task properties that can maximize the reliability of this new assessment tool for the detection of memory impairments in prodromal cases of AD.

Introduction

Memory assessment in individuals at risk of Alzheimer's disease (AD), such as those with Mild Cognitive Impairment (MCI), has long focused on episodic memory functions (Fields, Ferman, Boeve, & Smith, 2011; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, & Della Sala, 2010; Parra et al., 2011). Examples are Paired Associates Learning (PAL) tasks (Sahakian et al., 1988), the Face Name Associative Memory Exam (FNAME) (Amariglio et al., 2012, Rentz et al., 2013), the Free and Cued Selective Reminding test (FCSRT) (Barbeau et al., 2004; Buschke, 2014; Grober, Buschke, Crystal, Bang, & Dresner, 1988; Sarazin et al., 2007), and other episodic memory tests (Ivanoiu et al., 2005). These tests are known to assess functions of the hippocampus which are essential to episodic memory formation i.e., associative memory (Tulving, 2002). Tests assessing associative memory functions of the hippocampus are considered markers for AD (Auriacombe et al., 2010, Barbeau et al., 2008, Dubois et al., 2010, Rentz et al., 2013, Sarazin et al., 2007). To uphold the claim that the associative function is that selectively impaired in AD, it is necessary to demonstrate that such impairments are greater than those found when patients remember the individual items. For instance, memory for faces (Sperling et al., 2003), lists of words (Gallo, Sullivan, Daffner, Schacter, & Budson, 2004), or locations (Stehli, Chubb, & Jacob, 2003), are functions affected by AD. This makes it difficult to ascertain that holding associations between these items in memory (e.g., faces and locations, faces and names) is the hallmark of AD. This is important because item memory and associative memory dissociate (Chalfonte, Verfaellie, Johnson, & Reiss, 1996; Old & Naveh-Benjamin, 2008) and the form of representation claimed to be specifically affected by the hippocampal amnesia of AD is the latter. This caveat i.e., limited underlying constructs, has been recently highlighted by a recent consensus paper (Costa et al., 2017).

The Visual Short-Term Memory Binding Test (VSTMBT) was developed to investigate if the function responsible for binding features within object representations is affected by AD above and beyond that supporting single feature processing (Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Mendez, et al., 2010). The test assesses participants’ memory for single features such as shapes and for combination of features such as shape-color bindings. When memory load is controlled for (i.e., via titration to keep patients’ and controls’ memory performance for individual features at the same level), patients with AD show memory binding deficits which are far greater than those found when memory for single features is assessed (Della Sala, Parra, Fabi, Luzzi, & Abrahams, 2012; Parra et al., 2009; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, & Della Sala, 2010; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Mendez, et al., 2010; Parra et al., 2011). Such specific increase of the cost of binding has been observed since the preclinical stages of AD. This fits well current trends in the assessment of AD which have shifted toward a new lexicon (Costa et al., 2017, Dubois et al., 2010, Dubois et al., 2016) that encourages the detection of subtle cognitive impairments in stages prior to dementia. The VSTMBT detects such early impairments, even when other novel and traditional tests have failed (Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Mendez, et al., 2010; Parra et al., 2011).

The results form a recent study (Koppara et al., 2015) suggest that memory load may be a factor precluding the specificity of the VSTMBT (i.e., greater cost of binding in patients than in controls). Previous studies have manipulated memory load by presenting patients and controls with a different number of to-be-remembered items (Della Sala, Kozlova, Stamate, & Parra, 2018; Della Sala et al., 2012; Parra et al., 2009; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, & Della Sala, 2010; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Mendez, et al., 2010; Parra et al., 2011). Such manipulation rested on the assumption that VSTM stores a limited number of items (Luck & Vogel, 1997; Vogel, Woodman, & Luck, 2001) and that increasing the number of items above such a limit (i.e., 4) would overload memory, rendering the task more challenging and performance poorer. Titration aimed at reducing differences at baseline (i.e., memory for single features). This led to suggesting that patients with AD present with a selective deficit of VSTMB. (Koppara et al., 2015) showed that without titration (i.e., patients and controls tested with the same visual arrays of 3 items), the selectivity of the VSTMBT holds for people with Subjective Cognitive Deficits (SCD) but not for people with MCI. Considering that memory binding is maintained to be selectively impaired in AD and that MCI is an uncertain clinical category which holds limited value to predict future risk of dementia, it is important to demonstrate the precise testing conditions with which selective impairments of VSTMB can be found. Is the specific impaired ability to binding features in VSTM that has been considered a hallmark of AD. Hence, identifying such a hallmark in MCI people might provide more reliable evidence of AD pathology as the likely underlying mechanism. To address these outstanding issues, in the present paper we present data from groups of healthy older adults and people with MCI who were assessed with the VSTMBT using arrays of 2 and 3 items and without titration. If the above-mentioned selectivity is contingent upon memory load, it would be observed only under the low memory load condition (i.e., 2 items).

Section snippets

Participants

Participants came from two separate samples of people with MCI and matched controls assessed with different versions of the VSTMBT. One sample was tested with a version of the task presenting 2 items, the other sample was assessed with a version presenting 3 items. Table 1 shows the demographic, clinical and neuropsychological variables of the participants tested with the two set sizes. All participants underwent neuropsychological assessment. People with MCI met criteria proposed by Petersen

Results

The groups were matched on age, education, and depression scores. People with MCI showed a profile compatible with the multi-domain amnestic stage. The two groups of people with MCI showed a very similar profile of cognitive impairments. Control groups from the two samples did not differ in any of the neuropsychological scores (see Table 1).

The ANOVA model revealed a main effect of Group [F(3,100) = 24.9, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.43; β = 1.0], main effect of Condition [F(1,100) = 187.14, p < 0.001, η2

Discussion

The present study was carried out to investigate whether and under what condition people with MCI present with the typical pattern of VSTMB impairments consistently found in patients with AD dementia. We were driven by the need of providing evidence of the task's psychometric features that can be clinically friendly as within these setting, procedures such as titration of task difficulty are unfeasible. We also sought evidence of whether VSTMB deficits in MCI are observed in those people who

Conflict of interest

We wish to confirm that there are no known conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgments

The studies presented here were supproted by Alzheimer's Society Grants AS-R42303 and AS-SF-14-008 awared to MAP in collaboration with SDS and SFG. The support from the Alzheimer's Scotland Dementia Research Centre and the Centre for Cognitive Ageing and Cognitive Epidemiology part of the cross council Lifelong Health and Wellbeing Initiative (MR/K026992/1) both from the University of Edinburgh is also acknowledged.

References (61)

  • R.A. Sperling et al.

    Toward defining the preclinical stages of Alzheimer's disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease

    Alzheimer's & Dementia

    (2011)
  • S.M. Stark et al.

    A task to assess behavioral pattern separation (BPS) in humans: Data from healthy aging and mild cognitive impairment

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • D.A. Wolk et al.

    Recollection and familiarity in amnestic mild cognitive impairment: A global decline in recognition memory

    Neuropsychologia

    (2008)
  • J.A. Yesavage et al.

    Development and validation of a geriatric depression screening scale: A preliminary report

    Journal of Psychiatric Research

    (1982)
  • R.E. Amariglio et al.

    Validation of the Face Name Associative Memory Exam in cognitively normal older individuals

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (2012)
  • S. Auriacombe et al.

    Validity of the free and cued selective reminding test in predicting dementia: The 3C study

    Neurology

    (2010)
  • E. Barbeau et al.

    Evaluation of visual recognition memory in MCI patients

    Neurology

    (2004)
  • I.J. Bennett et al.

    Limbic tract integrity contributes to pattern separation performance across the lifespan

    Cerebral Cortex

    (2015)
  • G. Blessed et al.

    The association between quantitative measures of dementia and of senile change in the cerebral grey matter of elderly subjects

    The British Journal of Psychiatry

    (1968)
  • H. Buschke

    Rationale of the memory binding test

  • M.A. Cecchini et al.

    Recalling feature bindings differentiates Alzheimer's disease from frontotemporal dementia

    Journal of Neurology

    (2017)
  • B.L. Chalfonte et al.

    Spatial location memory in amnesia: Binding item and location information under incidental and intentional encoding conditions

    Memory

    (1996)
  • N. Cowan

    The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why?

    Current Directions in Psychological Science

    (2010)
  • D.C. Delis et al.

    California verbal learning test: Form II

    (1987)
  • S. Della Sala et al.

    A transcultural cognitive marker of Alzheimer’s Disease

    International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry

    (2018)
  • M. Didic et al.

    Which memory system is impaired first in Alzheimer's disease?

    Journal of Alzheimer's Disease

    (2011)
  • J.M. Doherty et al.

    Resource-sharing in multiple-component working memory

    Memory & Cognition

    (2016)
  • G. Fernández et al.

    Visual processing during short-term memory binding in mild Alzheimer's disease

    Journal of Alzheimer's Disease

    (2018)
  • J.A. Fields et al.

    Neuropsychological assessment of patients with dementing illness

    Nature Reviews Neurology

    (2011)
  • D.A. Gallo et al.

    Associative recognition in Alzheimer's disease: Evidence for impaired recall-to-reject

    Neuropsychology

    (2004)
  • Cited by (26)

    • Deficits in short-term memory binding are detectable in individuals with brain amyloid deposition in the absence of overt neurodegeneration in the Alzheimer's disease continuum

      2021, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      The test showed high sensitivity and specificity for sporadic AD (Cecchini et al., 2020, 2017; Della Sala, Kozlova, Stamate, & Parra, 2016; Kozlova, Parra, Titova, Gantman, & Della Sala, 2020; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Méndez, et al., 2010) and familial AD due to the presenilin-1 E280A mutation (Liang et al., 2016; Norton et al., 2020; Parra, Abrahams, Logie, Méndez, et al., 2010; Parra, Saarimäki, et al., 2015). It has also been shown to differentiate amnestic mild cognitive impairment (MCI) from controls (Cecchini et al., 2020; Koppara et al., 2015; Kozlova et al., 2020; Parra et al., 2019; Parra, Mikulan, et al., 2017; Pietto et al., 2016; Valdés Hernández et al., 2020). In a different study evaluating carriers of the presenilin-1 E280A mutation, Parra et al. (Parra et al., 2011) found that the STMB was the only cognitive test (compared to Paired Associates Learning, Rey Figure test, verbal fluencies, Trail Making test and Mini-mental State Examination) that could discriminate controls from asymptomatic carriers of the mutation in analyses using receiver operating characteristic curves (ROC), with an area under de curve (AUC) equal to 0.86.

    • The striatum, the hippocampus, and short-term memory binding: Volumetric analysis of the subcortical grey matter's role in mild cognitive impairment

      2020, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      The patient who features in this case study failed all the relational memory tests but succeeded on all the conjunctive memory tests. These studies bolster a growing body of work that challenges the notion that hippocampi are the key neurological loci for relational but not conjunctive STM binding (Parra et al., 2019; Parra et al., 2015). Furthermore, they also challenge the notion that the hippocampus and its associated functions should be the target of cognitive assessments aimed at the early detection of AD (Dubois et al., 2010; Dubois et al., 2016a,b).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text