Chapter Four - Consequences of Thought Speed

https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2017.10.003Get rights and content

Abstract

The speed of thinking is a frequently overlooked aspect of mental life. However, the pace of thought is an essential property of thinking, and its consequences have recently begun to be discovered. In this chapter, we review the psychological consequences of accelerated and decelerated thought pace. We begin by examining how the manipulation of thought speed alters mood, self-perception, risk-taking, creativity, and arousal. We highlight the energizing, activating, and hedonic effects of fast thinking, and we show how thought-speed effects are independent of thought content, fluency, and goal progress. We describe an adaptive theory of thought speed wherein psychological responses to the acceleration of thinking confer adaptive advantages for confronting novel, urgent, and rapidly changing situations, and engaging in behaviors driven by appetitive motivation. Lastly, we discuss implications of thought speed and its manipulation for treatment of mental illness, for design and delivery of communications and messages, and for life in the age of rapid access and exposure to information.

Introduction

We may never have met, but there is something I can say about you with complete certainty: Right now, at this very moment, you are thinking. There is no way that you could be reading this without doing it. You probably could go hours or even days without eating, drinking, speaking, or even checking e-mail, but you could not go that long without thinking. Even when your mind wanders to no place special, you are thinking. Even when you try not to think about anything, you are thinking (perhaps about trying not to think). Nothing may be more fundamental to human existence than the ongoing action of the mind. So thought Descartes who, in what is likely the most famous line in the history of philosophy, claimed: “I think, therefore I am.”

Not surprisingly, then, people devote a good deal of attention to the content of their thoughts. They tell others what they think about topics ranging from politics, to reality TV, to where to get a good meatball sandwich. They choose friendships, careers, and vacation destinations after first consulting their thoughts on which choice to make. People even analyze their dreams, looking for meaning in thoughts that emerge when they are not controlling them. The research that we review herein, though, speaks to another aspect of thinking—one that has more recently become an avenue for examining the nature of thought. That aspect involves not WHAT we think about, but rather HOW we think about it. In particular, the specific case of our work, and other work related to it, investigates the consequences of not the content of our thoughts but rather the SPEED, or the pace, with which we have those thoughts.

It has long been known that thoughts with positive content make people happy compared to thoughts with negative content (e.g., Velten, 1968). Children are taught the old trick, memorialized in the musical The Sound of Music, of trying to alleviate a sad mood by thinking of their “favorite things” (Wise et al., 1965). The past decade of research on thought speed suggests a new trick for alleviating a sad mood: thinking about things really fast. Although Rodgers and Hammerstein did not write about it, thinking fast about favorite things like “raindrops on roses” and “whiskers on kittens” alleviates sadness far better than thinking slowly about those very same things. It is this pair of simple insights, that the pace of thought can be accelerated and that such accelerations impact mood, that provides the starting point for our investigations of the consequences of thought speed. But before describing this fundamental speed–mood connection, we turn to a brief discussion of the thought-speed concept.

Thought speed in its essence involves the number of thoughts that one is having per unit of time. Until recently, the idea of thought speed came up only rarely in scientific discussions—and was not a variable that was manipulated; consequently, its effects were not measured or known. The idea of thought speed as an important variable is not entirely new, though. In the psychiatric literature, associations between various mental disorders and the speed of thought have long been noted. Individuals experiencing mania, for example, typically exhibit the hallmark symptoms of “racing thoughts” and “flight of ideas” (Hanwella & de Silva, 2011; Mansell & Pedley, 2008). Indeed, this symptom may be even more common than the stereotypic manic symptom of euphoric affect in predicting the onset of a manic episode (Keitner et al., 1996; Mansell & Pedley, 2008; Molnar, Feeney, & Fava, 1988). Individuals suffering from depressive episodes experience the opposite end of the thought-speed spectrum, with their thoughts sometimes slowed to the point of feeling immobilized (Caligiuri & Ellwanger, 2000). Another psychiatric disorder, ADHD or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder, also has been associated with abnormal thought speed, with at least some of those experiencing it having “sluggish cognitive tempo” (Becker, 2013).

Thought speed has also sometimes been recognized as a feature or symptom of experiences not involving psychiatric disorder. Certain mind-altering drugs, for example, are known to make thoughts race, including stimulant drugs such as cocaine and amphetamines (Asghar, Tanay, Baker, Greenshaw, & Silverstone, 2003; Heilbronner & Meck, 2014; Kirkpatrick et al., 2016; Vollm et al., 2004), as well as more pedestrian drugs such as caffeine and nicotine (Childs & de Wit, 2006; Durlach, Edmunds, Howard, & Tipper, 2002; Edwards, Wesnes, Warburton, & Gale, 1985; Hinton & Meck, 1996; Smith, 2002; Smit & Rogers, 2000; Warburton & Mancuso, 1998). Also, in everyday life, people experience fluctuations in their thought speed even when mental disorder and drugs are not part of the picture. For example, drinking the first cup of coffee in the morning may be a reaction to the unpleasant feeling that one's thoughts are too slow. Lying awake at night with a bout of insomnia, one may lament that one's thoughts will not slow down. On the other hand, when participating in a productive brainstorming session, one may instead delight in the feeling of one's thoughts racing along.

In each of the above examples of fast and slow thought speed, the pace of thought seems to be a symptom of some other condition—whether it be mental illness, drug intake, tiredness, insomnia, or creative excitement. However, we would argue—and have begun to show in our experimental work—that thought speed is important not only as a symptom in psychological life but also a cause of various aspects of psychological life. By manipulating thought speed through controlled experimentation, we demonstrate that the pace of thought has wide-ranging consequences. This research development is particularly important because thought speed is quite amenable to alteration—and fluctuates and varies during the course of everyday human experience.

In this chapter, we begin by reviewing basic evidence for a causal link between positive mood and the speed of thinking. From there, we discuss other consequences of thought speed, most notably consequences for self-perception, risk-taking, creativity, and mental health. Following this review, we offer a theoretical account to explain why thought speed impacts these various important aspects of psychological life. Finally, we discuss the importance of thought speed in the context of modern life, where the speed of exposure to stimuli—especially digital information and communications—seems to be accelerating more every day.

In reviewing this material, we present an inclusive review of consequences of thought speed as well as a theoretical framework for those effects. We describe how thought speed, thought content, and behavior covary in predictable patterns. Fast thinking is associated with what might be called an activated state: positive mood, energy, arousal, confidence, risk-taking, approach behaviors, and problem-solving. We describe a variety of ways in which thought speed can be experimentally manipulated to produce changes in affect, perception, and behavior. We also describe psychophysiological mechanisms that may facilitate the connections between speed, thinking, and behavior, including activation of the dopaminergic system and the phenomena of embodiment and entrainment. In this review, we discuss the evidence for these findings, as well as our ongoing experimental research program on thought speed. We also discuss implications of this work for treatment of mental illness (an area where experiments from our laboratory have begun to offer some promise) and for modern social life.

Section snippets

Thought Speed Affects Mood and Emotion

Most of us will never know firsthand what it feels like to be climbing an icy mountain, lose our footing, start falling hundreds of feet, and think we are about to die. Researchers Noyes and Kletti (1972) found something surprising in the accounts of hundreds of mountain climbers (and others) who have had near-death experiences: Rather than feeling despair, they often reported feeling oddly joyful and euphoric. Almost all of them reported that they experienced an acceleration of thinking when

More Consequences of Thought Speed

Initial experiments investigating the effects of thought speed were primarily focused on emotion, but continued research on thought speed has revealed a host of other consequences of manipulating the pace of human thought. We now review a number of these other consequences. These include effects of experimentally manipulated thought pace on behavior (e.g., risk-taking), self-perception (e.g., self-esteem), and creativity (e.g., problem-solving). After reviewing these wide-ranging effects, we

Thought Speed and Related Constructs

In this section, we review a number of constructs that seem related to thought speed and discuss those relations. These include fluency, System 1 thinking, and goal progress.

How Thought Speed Works

Why does thought speed exert a wide range of effects on human emotion, judgment, and action? In this section, we present an adaptive theory of thought speed that aims to explain these relationships.

Thought Speed and Treatment for Depression

Depressive disorders are mood disorders that are characterized by anhedonia, the loss of interest in activities that were once pleasurable, feelings of sadness, worthlessness, loss of energy, loss of motivation, and physiological symptoms such as disruptions of sleep and appetite (DSM-5, American Psychiatric Association, 2013). In the 1960s, Beck's Cognitive Theory of Depression (e.g., Beck, 1963) revolutionized the conceptualization and the treatment of depression. Beck identified regular

Methods of Manipulating Thought Speed

Thought speed has been successfully manipulated using various methods. Below, we outline major categories of speed manipulations, including direct (e.g., timed stimuli) and indirect manipulations with substantial evidence to support their effectiveness as speed manipulations (e.g., time pressure, sympathetic arousal). Table 2 displays speed induction techniques that have been used in experimental research.

Some Future Directions for Thought-Speed Research

The adaptive theory of thought speed provides a framework for predicting the varied consequences of accelerating and decelerating thought speed. Some key causal relations in this theory have been tested, including the effects of thought speed on mood and risk-taking. However, as elaborated below, several key relations predicted by the model need to be further explored through empirical research.

Conclusion: Thought Speed in the Modern World

In this chapter, we reviewed a frequently overlooked dimension of the mind: the experience of speed in thinking. We may not be aware of the pace of our thinking until we experience sudden shifts, such as the malaise of being sick or a sudden inspiration from a flow of ideas. But thought speed, and mundane alterations in it, are a constant property of our human existence.

The idea of “today's fast-paced world” seems like an apt description of contemporary society in the industrialized world.

References (171)

  • S.R. Heilbronner et al.

    Dissociations between interval timing and intertemporal choice following administration of fluoxetine, cocaine, or methamphetamine

    Behavioral Processes

    (2014)
  • I. Hickie et al.

    Neo-striatal rCBF correlates of psychomotor slowing in patients with major depression

    Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging

    (1999)
  • G.C. Homish et al.

    Predictors of later bipolar disorder in patients with subthreshold symptoms

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2013)
  • G.I. Keitner et al.

    Prodromal and residual symptoms in bipolar I disorder

    Comprehensive Psychiatry

    (1996)
  • S. Khalfa et al.

    Role of tempo entrainment in psychophysiological differentiation of happy and sad music?

    International Journal of Psychophysiology

    (2008)
  • W. Mansell et al.

    The ascent into mania: A review of the psychological processes associated with the development of manic symptoms

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2008)
  • G.E. Mead et al.

    Mental Health and Physical Activity

    (2009)
  • W.H. Meck

    Neuropsychology of timing and time perception

    Brain and Cognition

    (2005)
  • M. Akinola et al.

    The dark side of creativity: Biological vulnerability and negative emotions lead to greater artistic creativity

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2008)
  • A.L. Alter et al.

    Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation

    Personality and Social Psychology Review

    (2009)
  • T.M. Amabile

    The social psychology of creativity: A consensual assessment technique

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1982)
  • T.M. Amabile et al.

    Perspectives on the social psychology of creativity

    The Journal of Creative Behavior

    (2012)
  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)
  • N.C. Andreasen

    Creativity and mental illness: Prevalence rates in writers and their first-degree relatives

    American Journal of Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • H. Andrews

    The effects of thought speed on consumer purchasing attitudes and behavior

    (2011)
  • D. Ariely et al.

    An experimental analysis of ending rules in Internet auctions

    RAND Journal of Economics

    (2005)
  • S.J. Asghar et al.

    Relationship of plasma amphetamine levels to physiological, subjective, cognitive, and biochemical measures in healthy volunteers

    Human Psychopharmacology

    (2003)
  • M. Baas et al.

    A meta-analysis of 25 years of mood-creativity research: Hedonic tone, activation, or regulatory focus?

    Psychological Bulletin

    (2008)
  • J.A. Bargh et al.

    Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • A.T. Beck

    Thinking and depression: Idiosyncratic content and cognitive distortions

    Archives of General Psychiatry

    (1963)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Cognitive therapy of depression

    (1979)
  • A.T. Beck et al.

    Manual for the Beck Depression Inventory-II

    (1996)
  • S.P. Becker

    Topical review: Sluggish cognitive tempo: Research findings and relevance for pediatric psychology

    Journal of Pediatric Psychology

    (2013)
  • J.R. Blackburn et al.

    Dopamine and preparatory behavior: II. A neurochemical analysis

    Behavioral Neuroscience

    (1989)
  • H.R. Bosker

    How our own speech rate influences our perception of others

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition

    (2017)
  • E.M. Bowden et al.

    Normative data for 144 compound remote associate problems

    Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers

    (2003)
  • C. Breinstein et al.

    The contribution of speech rate and pitch variation to the perception of vocal emotions in a German and an American sample

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2001)
  • J. Brisswalter et al.

    Effects of acute physical exercise characteristics on cognitive performance

    Sports Medicine

    (2002)
  • C.V. Buhusi et al.

    What makes us tick? Functional and neural mechanisms of interval timing

    Nature Reviews Neuroscience

    (2005)
  • J.P. Byrnes et al.

    Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1999)
  • C.S. Carver et al.

    Origins and functions of positive and negative affect: A control-process view

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1990)
  • S. Chaiken

    Heuristic versus systematic information processing and the use of source versus message cues in persuasion

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1980)
  • J.J. Chandler et al.

    Fast thought speed induces risk taking

    Psychological Science

    (2012)
  • J. Chebat et al.

    Voice and persuasion in a banking telemarketing context

    Perceptual and Motor Skills

    (2007)
  • E. Childs et al.

    Subjective, behavioral, and physiological effects of acute caffeine in light, nondependent caffeine users

    Psychopharmacology

    (2006)
  • J.I. Davis et al.

    How does facial feedback modulate emotional experience?

    Journal of Research in Personality

    (2009)
  • C.G. DeYoung et al.

    Cognitive abilities involved in insight problem solving: An individual differences model

    Creativity Research Journal

    (2008)
  • B. Duff et al.

    Think fast, feel good? Thought speed enhances mood and product trial

    Advances in Consumer Research

    (2008)
  • B.R.L. Duff et al.

    Is there a need for speed? Fast animation as context increases product trial intent and self-focus

    International Journal of Advertising

    (2015)
  • K. Duncker

    On problem solving

    Psychological Monographs

    (1945)
  • View full text