Research report
Lesions of the perirhinal cortex impair sensory preconditioning in rats

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00168-6Get rights and content

Abstract

The effects of lesions of the perirhinal cortex on the development of associations between two conditioned stimuli (CSs) were examined with a sensory preconditioning procedure. Rats were given either bilateral electrolytic lesions of the perirhinal cortex or control surgery. They were then given either paired or unpaired presentations of a light CS and a tone CS. All of the rats were then given eyeblink conditioning procedures that involved paired presentations of either the light or tone and a periorbital shock unconditioned stimulus (US). The rats were finally given a test session that consisted of unpaired presentations of the tone and light CSs. Sensory preconditioning was established in the control group, but not in the lesion group. The findings are consistent with the view that the perirhinal cortex is involved in forming associations between neutral stimuli (even in the absence of reinforcement).

Introduction

A great deal of progress has been made toward understanding the neurobiology of simple associative learning [1], [9], [11], [16], [19], [24], [25], [26], [29], [39], [42], [49]. Classical conditioning procedures can be used to establish simple associative learning by pairing a conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US). Sensory preconditioning is a special case of simple associative learning because it involves an initial pairing of two conditioned stimuli (CS1 and CS2 in phase 1) in the absence of an unconditioned stimulus, after which the subject is trained in traditional classical conditioning (CS1-US in phase 2) [5]. The presence of an association between CS1 and CS2 can only be inferred by post-training test presentations of CS2 (phase 3). Previous studies have shown that animals given initial CS1-CS2 pairings before CS1-US training respond to test presentations of CS2, whereas control groups given unpaired presentations of CS1 and CS2 do not [36], [37], [47], [48]. It is thought that the animals given paired training respond to CS2 by virtue of its association with the excitatory CS1 [5], [48]. The two conditioned stimuli often differ in modality (e.g. tone and light) and thus require the formation of a crossmodal association.

It is difficult to specifically distinguish between disruptions of encoding or retrieval because of their intimate relationship [38], [50]. However, the clearly delineated encoding and retrieval opportunities for a CS1-CS2 association in sensory preconditioning (i.e. phase 1 and phase 3, respectively) enable a more focused study of the underlying neural bases of sensory associations.

Previous studies of the neural mechanisms of sensory preconditioning demonstrated that lesions of non-specific association cortex [47], the fimbria [36], or field CA1 of the hippocampus [37] prevented the establishment of sensory preconditioning. Based on these studies, a reasonable conclusion is that the connectivity between the hippocampus and cerebral cortex must remain intact for sensory preconditioning to be established.

The cortical areas surrounding the hippocampus in the rat have been divided into postrhinal, perirhinal, and entorhinal cortices [8]. The perirhinal and postrhinal cortices both provide input to the entorhinal cortex. However, in the rat, the perirhinal cortex receives substantial input from both auditory and visual cortical areas, whereas the postrhinal cortex receives input primarily from visual cortex [7], [10]. This connectivity establishes the perirhinal cortex as a major source of polysensory information for the subiculum and hippocampus via substantial connections with the lateral entorhinal cortex [8], [32], [40]. Thus, the perirhinal efferents to the hippocampus may provide the necessary information for crossmodal sensory associations. There is precedence for the view that the hippocampus and parahippocampal region constitute functionally distinct components of a larger system [6], [12], [13], [14], [33], [43] or are components of overlapping, but separate systems [15], [20], [21], [22], [28], [30], [35], [51].

It has recently been proposed that the parahippocampal region binds the elements of paired associates, whereas the relations among the paired elements are dependent on hippocampal processing [12]. In context of sensory preconditioning, it is possible that the perirhinal cortex functions in the encoding or storage of the CS1-CS2 association, which is flexibly expressed by the paired group in the testing phase.

The current study used a sensory preconditioning procedure to examine the role of the perirhinal cortex in the development of associations between two conditioned stimuli. Rats were given either bilateral lesions of the perirhinal cortex or control surgery followed one week later by three phases of training. In Phase 1, half of the rats from each surgical group were given paired presentations of a tone CS and a light CS. The other half of the rats were given unpaired presentations of the two CSs. In Phase 2, all of the rats were given eyeblink conditioning procedures that involved paired presentations of either the light or tone and a periorbital shock US. In Phase 3, all of the rats were given a test session that consisted of unpaired presentations of the tone and light CSs (Table 1).

Section snippets

Subjects

Subjects were 27 male Long–Evans rats (200–250 g). The rats were housed in the animal colony in Spence Laboratories at the University of Iowa. All rats were maintained on a 12-h light, 12-h dark photoperiod, with light onset at 06:30 h. The rats were assigned to one of two surgical groups (control or lesion) and one of two training conditions (paired or unpaired, see Section 2.4 below).

Surgery

One week prior to training, rats were removed from their home cage and anesthetized by an i.p. injection of

Behaviour

The rats did not exhibit CRs during the first phase of training. All rats used in this study reached the performance criterion of 80% CRs during training phase 2 (Fig. 1). Sensory preconditioning was evident in the rats given control surgery, but not in the rats given lesions of the perirhinal cortex. As Fig. 2 shows, control rats given paired presentations of the tone and light in phase 1 responded to the non-reinforced stimulus (CS2) more than control rats given unpaired presentations of the

Discussion

The present study investigated the effects of lesions of the perirhinal cortex on the formation of associations between two conditioned stimuli using a sensory preconditioning procedure. Sensory preconditioning was established in the control group receiving initial paired presentations of CS1 and CS2. Sensory preconditioning was not established in either lesion group nor in the control group receiving initial unpaired presentations of CS1 and CS2.

There are at least three possible roles for the

Acknowledgements

The authors thank Dr Mark E. Stanton for providing the eyeblink conditioning equipment.

References (52)

  • R.D. Burwell et al.

    Cortical afferents of the perirhinal, postrhinal, and entorhinal cortices of the rat

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1998)
  • R.D. Burwell et al.

    Perirhinal and postrhinal cortices of the rat: interconnectivity and connections with the entorhinal cortex

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1998)
  • M. Davis

    The role of the amygdala in conditioned fear

  • T.W. Deacon et al.

    Afferent connections of the perirhinal cortex in the rat

    J. Comp. Neurol.

    (1983)
  • J.F. Disterhoft et al.

    Conditioning-specific membrane changes of rabbit hippocampal neurons measured in vitro

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1986)
  • H. Eichenbaum et al.

    Two functional components of the hippocampal memory system

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1994)
  • H. Eichenbaum et al.

    On the binding of associations in memory: clues from studies on the role of the hippocampal region in paired-associate learning

    Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci.

    (1995)
  • H. Eichenbaum et al.

    Functional organization of the hippocampal memory system

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1996)
  • J.H. Freeman et al.

    Disruption of cerebellar maturation by an antimitotic agent impairs the ontogeny of eyeblink conditioning in rats

    J. Neurosci.

    (1995)
  • M. Gabriel et al.

    Hippocampal control of cingulate cortical and anterior thalamic information processing during learning in rabbits

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1987)
  • M. Gabriel

    Discriminative avoidance learning: a model system approach

  • D. Gaffan

    Dissociated effects of perirhinal cortex ablation, fornix transection and amygdalectomy: evidence for multiple memory systems in the primate temporal lobe

    Exp. Brain Res.

    (1994)
  • D. Gaffan et al.

    Interaction of perirhinal cortex with the fornix-fimbria: memory for objects and ‘object-in-place’ memory

    J. Neurosci.

    (1996)
  • M.J. Glenn et al.

    Place memory is intact in rats with perirhinal cortex lesions

    Behav. Neurosci.

    (1998)
  • E. Kang et al.

    Hippocampal modulation of cingulo-thalamic neuronal activity and discriminative avoidance learning in rabbits

    Hippocampus

    (1998)
  • B.S. Kapp et al.

    A neuroanatomical systems analysis of conditioned bradycardia in the rabbit

  • Cited by (53)

    • The neural substrates of higher-order conditioning: A review

      2022, Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews
      Citation Excerpt :

      Pairings of an auditory (or visual) S2 and a visual (or auditory) S1 produce an association that requires activity in a network of cortical regions including the retrosplenial (RSP), posterior parietal (PPC) and perirhinal cortices (PRh). Rats subjected to electrolytic lesions of the RSP, PPC or PRh and exposed to pairings of S2 and S1 acquire responding to S1 across its subsequent pairings with a US (appetitive or aversive) but fail to respond when tested with its S2-associate (Robinson et al., 2011; Robinson and Bucci, 2012; Nicholson and Freeman, 2000). More recently, studies using chemogenetic or pharmacological interventions have confirmed that the RSP and PRh support formation of the S2-S1 association (Robinson et al., 2014; Holmes et al., 2013, 2018).

    • Avoidance of learnt fear: Models, potential mechanisms, and future directions

      2022, Behaviour Research and Therapy
      Citation Excerpt :

      Rodent studies in higher-order conditioning have shown that different brain circuits are activated during presentation of a higher-order CS, either before CS+ obtains threat value (i.e., sensory preconditioning) or after CS+ obtains threat value (i.e., second-order conditioning). In sensory preconditioning, the hippocampus and the adjacent perirhinal cortex, but not the basolateral amygdala (BLA), are responsible for associating the neutral higher-order CS and the first-order CS together (Eichenbaum, Otto, & Cohen, 1992; Murray & Bussey, 1999; Nicholson & Freeman, 2000; Talk, Gandhi, & Matzel, 2002; Wimmer & Shohamy, 2011; Winters, Saksida, & Bussey, 2008), and play an important role in transferring the newly learnt value of the first-order CS to the higher-order CS after fear conditioning. In second-order conditioning, both the hippocampus and BLA are responsible for associating a neutral second-order CS with a fear-related first-order CS (Gewirtz & Davis, 1997; Gilboa, Sekeres, Moscovitch, & Winocur, 2014; Parkes & Westbrook, 2010).

    • Appetitive and aversive sensory preconditioning in rats is impaired by disruption of the postrhinal cortex

      2021, Neurobiology of Learning and Memory
      Citation Excerpt :

      Thus, regardless of whether stimulus-stimulus associations were formed between static or phasic stimuli or whether the revaluation of the paired stimulus occurred through association with an aversive or an appetitive US, the effects were the same; POR lesions disrupted the ability to use higher-order conditioned stimuli to guide prospective behavior. Overall, these results situate POR within a broad network of brain structures such as the hippocampus (Iordanova et al., 2011), fimbria (Port and Patterson, 1984), perirhinal cortex (Nicholson and Freeman, 2000) and retrosplenial cortex (Robinson et al., 2011; 2014; 2018) that are also recruited during higher-order conditioning. After sustaining permanent damage to the POR, rats were unable to use higher-order conditioned contextual information to guide future behavior, yet POR-lesioned rats maintained the ability to distinguish between two contexts that were experienced through first-order fear conditioning.

    • Neuronal Computation Underlying Inferential Reasoning in Humans and Mice

      2020, Cell
      Citation Excerpt :

      This ability to infer previously unobserved relationships is thought to be critical for flexible and adaptive behavior. Anatomical lesions in rodents and functional imaging in humans have started to uncover the macroscopic network of brain regions supporting inferential decisions (Bunsey and Eichenbaum, 1996; Hampton et al., 2006; Jones et al., 2012; Nicholson and Freeman, 2000; Preston et al., 2004; Robinson et al., 2014; Wimmer and Shohamy, 2012; Zeithamova et al., 2012a), highlighting the involvement of orbitofrontal, medial prefrontal, perirhinal, and retrosplenial cortices, along with the hippocampus. However, the mechanistic contribution of these regions and the neuronal computation underpinning inference remain unclear.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text