The “Mute” Sex Revisited: Vocal Production and Perception Learning in Female Songbirds

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(03)33002-5Get rights and content

Publisher Summary

Songbirds are known for their vocal versatility and the great developmental plasticity that permits or even makes it normal that adult signals are shaped by social learning processes. The chapter describes the vocal perception learning and vocal production learning in female songbirds. Song functions as an important mate attraction signal. Hence, it is often sexually dimorphic, with males typically the vocally displaying and advertising sex. The vocal perception learning in female song birds depends on recognition, song preferences, and perpetual fine tuning. Bird vocalizations are traditionally divided into calls and songs. The categorization is based both on physical characteristics of the signal (calls are shorter and less complex than songs) and also on its function. The chapter describes song learning and call learning in female song birds. Female song seems rare in comparison, and these behavioral dimorphisms also seem to map onto neuroanatomical differences in the specialized brain nuclei involved in singing. Consequently, there has been an emphasis on studying song acquisition in male songbirds. However, with female song described in a growing number of species, a new interest in the form and function of female song has surged. Likewise, theory has spawned interest in causes of variation on the receiver's side but also increasingly in the extent and function of female ornaments. With a culturally transmitted mating signal, learned preferences arise as an additional dimension next to genetically inherited and condition-dependent variation in female preferences and signaling.

Introduction

In almost all songbird species studied males sing, but differences in female song range from species where females have never been observed to sing to those where females sing as much as males. In male songbirds, song functions both as a territorial signal and a mate-attracting signal (Catchpole and Slater, 1995), and thus song is under sexual selection through male-male competition and female choice (Andersson, 1994). In the majority of species studied, males have been described as the vocally displaying, females the choosing sex. All the true songbirds (oscines), which comprise about half of the extant bird species, acquire at least part of their song by imitation (Catchpole and Slater, 1995). They are, by far, the best studied taxon among those documented to be vocal learners (Janik and Slater, 1997). However, a closer inspection of the literature shows that it has almost exclusively addressed the subject of vocal learning in male oscines. For example, the absence of the subject of female vocal learning is striking in all the most recent comprehensive reviews on bird song (Kroodsma 1982, Kroodsma 1996, Catchpole 1995).

While it is undisputed that many species show clear sex differences in song usage, vocal learning in female songbirds has been so little studied that a sex difference in vocal learning is less certain. Much of bird song research effort has been biased towards temperate zone species where female song is supposed to be rare compared to the tropics (Kroodsma et al., 1996) or to Australia (Robinson, 1949). However, this view has also changed recently. Female song in temperate zones might be rare in terms of absolute time spent singing, but in almost all of the more intensively studied species, females are reported to sing at least occasionally (reviews and references, e.g., Nice 1943, Ringleben 1982, Ritchison 1983a, Langmore 1998). Recent observational and experimental studies have revised the notion that “mute” females occasionally produce functionless song when in hormonal disbalance, and a number of hypotheses have been brought forward regarding the possible function of female song (reviews in Ritchison 1983a, Langmore 1998). This recent interest in female song has also revealed that its acquisition is poorly understood. For almost all species where both sexes sing it is unclear whether and how females learn their songs and whether they learn the same way as males do. A thorough understanding of the behavioral sex differences is paramount to studying the covarying neurophysiology and anatomy, but it is also necessary to understand the intricate interplay in gene-culture co-evolutionary processes.

With a culturally transmitted mating signal, the question arises of the extent to which cultural transmission in the actively advertising sex is paralleled in the choosing one. Song is an important factor in female choice, with different song attributes varying in their relative importance across species (Andersson 1994, Catchpole 1995, Searcy 1996). Given that songs are culturally transmitted, perception of oscine vocalizations, like their production, should show some developmental plasticity. Learning processes on the receiver's side might be essential to the dynamics of a communication system relying on culturally transmitted signals. Evidence is accumulating that female preferences for specific variants of conspecific acoustic signals are indeed greatly influenced by learning. The fact that song is often sexually dimorphic offers the unique opportunity to disentangle production from perception learning and to identify specialized adaptations of the brain. This is of great interest from a neuroethological point of view, especially to those focusing on mechanisms of learning and memory (Nottebohm 1990, MacDougall-Shackleton 1999, Bolhuis 2003). However, there has also been a recent surge of interest into perception learning from those with a more functionally and evolutionary orientated approach to behavior. While learning processes have now been acknowledged to contribute to variation between males, several authors have recently stressed that imprinting-like processes could play a far more important role in forming female preference than previously assumed (Owens 1999, ten Cate 1999). Developmental influences on female preferences are little understood, but have been identified as important sources for within-population variation in female preference (Jennions and Petrie, 1997). Therefore, an important first step is to map the distribution and extent of its occurrence.

It seems timely to review what is currently known on how learning processes influence female songbird perception, as well as the extent to which females learn their song and calls. This may provide a starting point for a more informed discussion on how earlier consideration of sex differences in vocal learning might have been at least partly confounded with sex differences in vocal performance. In addition, I hope to provide enough evidence for auditory perception learning in female songbirds to stimulate more research into receiver learning, as mating decisions exert selection pressures that are likely to have played an important role both in origin and maintenance of vocal learning.

Section snippets

Vocal Perception Learning in Female Songbirds

Song is often addressed toward potential mates (Kroodsma and Byers, 1991). There is now ample evidence that male song influences female choices (reviewed in Searcy and Yasukawa, 1996) and there is a lot to choose among. In many species, males have repertoires and so sing more than one variant of the species-specific song. In repertoire species, although birds may share song types, no two individual vocal repertoires may be the same. Males will also differ in how much they sing and the way they

Vocal Production Learning in Female Songbirds

Bird vocalizations are traditionally divided into calls and songs. The categorization is based both on physical characteristics of the signal (calls are shorter and less complex than songs) and also on its function (see later). This distinction has sometimes caused confusion, but has still proven useful in describing vocal repertoires (for discussion see Catchpole and Slater, 1995). Generally, calls are described to be used by both sexes for communication all year round, whereas songs are more

Female Vocal Learning in Non-Oscine Birds

Oscine songbirds are not the only bird taxon for which vocal learning has been reported (Catchpole and Slater, 1995), but they are the best studied. In comparison, literature on the subject available in other taxa is rather scarce, but there are at least two orders for which vocal learning has been established (i.e., parrots [Psittaciformes] and hummingbirds [Apodiformes]), and this might also involve females where they vocalize. Male and female budgerigars (Melopsittacus undulatus) modify

Sex Differences in Learning?

The previous sections have provided ample evidence for female vocal learning. Sex differences in the amount of singing are well documented in many species, but does a sex difference in song output equal sex differences in vocal learning? To compare male and female learning abilities, the two sexes need to be raised and tested under the same conditions and with identical song exposure, as even small differences in the circumstances under which song learning takes place can change its outcome (

Summary

Songbirds are known for their vocal versatility and the great developmental plasticity that permits or even makes it normal that adult signals are shaped by social learning processes. Song functions as an important mate attraction signal. Hence, it is often sexually dimorphic, with males typically the vocally displaying and advertising sex. Female song seems rare in comparison, and these behavioral dimorphisms also seem to map onto neuroanatomical differences in the specialized brain nuclei

Acknowledgements

I would like to thank Vincent Janik, Carel ten Cate, and Peter Slater for many fruitful discussions on the subject of vocal learning. Carel ten Cate, Hans Slabbekoorn, Johan Bolhuis, Peter Slater, Tim Roper, and Naomi Langmore provided constructive and much appreciated comments on earlier versions of this manuscript.

References (173)

  • W.P.J Dittus et al.

    Effects of song tutoring and acoustic isolation on the song repertoires of cardinals

    Anim. Behav.

    (1969)
  • M Eens

    Understanding the complex song of the European starling: An integrated ethological approach

    Adv. Stud. Behav.

    (1997)
  • T.M Freeberg

    Assortative mating in captive cowbirds is predicted by social experience

    Anim. Behav.

    (1996)
  • T.M Freeberg

    The cultural transmission of courtship patterns in cowbirds, Molothrus ater

    Anim. Behav.

    (1998)
  • P.R Grant et al.

    Mating patterns of Darwin's finch hybrids determined by song and morphology

    Bio. J. Linn. Soc.

    (1997)
  • S.L Halkin

    Nest-vicinity song exchanges may coordinate biparental care of northern cardinals

    Anim. Behav.

    (1997)
  • V.M Janik et al.

    Vocal learning in mammals

    Adv. Stud. Behav.

    (1997)
  • V.M Janik et al.

    The different roles of social learning in vocal communication

    Anim. Behav.

    (2000)
  • J.W Kling et al.

    Development of song and reinforcing effects of song in female chaffinches

    Anim. Behav.

    (1977)
  • N.E Langmore

    Functions of duet and solo songs of female birds

    Trends Ecol. Evol.

    (1998)
  • H Lind et al.

    Female great tits can identify mates by song

    Anim. Behav.

    (1996)
  • S.A MacDougall-Shackleton et al.

    Comparative studies of sex differences in the song-control system of songbirds

    Trends Neurosci.

    (1999)
  • M Andersson

    “Sexual Selection.”

    (1994)
  • M.C Baker

    The behavioral response of female Nuttall's white-crowned sparrows to male song of natal and alien dialects

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    (1983)
  • M.C Baker et al.

    The biology of bird-song dialects

    Behav. Brain Sci.

    (1985)
  • M.C Baker et al.

    Early experience determines song dialect responsiveness of female sparrows

    Science

    (1981)
  • M.C Baker et al.

    The response of female mountain white-crowned sparrows to songs from their natal dialect and an alien dialect

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    (1982)
  • E Balaban

    Cultural and genetic variation in swamp sparrows (Melospiza georgiana) II. Behavioural salience of geographic song variants

    Behaviour

    (1988)
  • G.F Ball et al.

    Sex differences in songbirds 25 years later: What have we learned and where do we go?

    Microsc. Res. Tech.

    (2001)
  • M.R Ballintijn et al.

    Sex differences in the vocalizations and syrinx of the collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto)

    Auk

    (1997)
  • M.R Ballintijn et al.

    Vocal development and its differentiation in a non-songbird: The collared dove (Streptopelia decaocto)

    Behaviour

    (1997)
  • L.F Baptista

    Territorial, courtship and duet songs of the Cuban grassquit (Tiaris canora)

    J. Orn.

    (1978)
  • L.F Baptista et al.

    Song dialects and mate selection in montane white-crowned sparrows

    Auk

    (1982)
  • P.H Becker

    Der Einflub des Lernens auf einfache und komplexe Gesangsstrophen der Sumpfmeise (Parus palustris)

    J. Orn.

    (1978)
  • L.D Beletsky

    Vocalizations of female northern orioles

    Condor

    (1982)
  • B Bertram

    The vocal behaviour of the Indian hill mynah

    Anim. Behav. Monogr.

    (1970)
  • J.J Bolhuis et al.

    Bird brains and songs: Neural mechanisms of birdsong perception and memory

    Anim. Biol.

    (2003)
  • E.A Brenowitz

    Comparative approaches to the avian song system

    J. Neurobiol.

    (1997)
  • E.D Brown

    The role of song and vocal imitation among common crows (Corvus brachyrhynchus)

    Z. Tierpsychol.

    (1985)
  • E.D Brown et al.

    Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part III. Sex specificity and individual specificity of vocal parts in communal chorus and duet songs

    Behaviour

    (1991)
  • E.D Brown et al.

    Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part I. Vocal sharing within and among social groups

    Behaviour

    (1988)
  • E.D Brown et al.

    Song sharing in a group-living songbird, the Australian magpie, Gymnorhina tibicen. Part I. Vocal sharing within and among social-groups

    Behaviour

    (1988)
  • C.K Catchpole et al.

    “Bird Song: Biological Themes and Variations.”

    (1995)
  • G Chilton et al.

    Songs and sexual reponses of female white-crowned sparrows (Zonotrichia leucophrys) from a mixed dialect population

    Behaviour

    (1996)
  • G Chilton et al.

    Mate choice by female white-crowned sparrows in a mixed dialect population

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    (1990)
  • M.A Cunningham et al.

    Vocal learning in white-crowned sparrows: Sensitive phase and song dialects

    Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol.

    (1983)
  • J Cynx et al.

    Role of gender, season, and familiarity in discrimination of conspecific song by zebra finches (Taeniopygia guttata)

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA

    (1992)
  • V Depraz et al.

    Early tutoring and adult reproductive behaviour in female domestic canary (Serinus canaria)

    Anim. Cogn.

    (2000)
  • T.I Drǎgǎnoiu et al.

    Directional female preference for an exaggerated male trait in canary (Serinus canaria) song

    Proc. Roy. Soc. Lond. B

    (2002)
  • S.M Farabaugh

    The ecological and social significance of duetting

  • Cited by (151)

    • Infectious disease and cognition in wild populations

      2022, Trends in Ecology and Evolution
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text