Deficits in executive functions and motor coordination in children with frontal lobe epilepsy

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00130-0Get rights and content

Abstract

Frontal lobe dysfunction in adults has been associated with impairments of planning abilities, working memory, impulse control, attention and certain aspects of motor coordination. However, very few studies have attempted to assess these functions in children suffering from frontal lobe epilepsy. The aim of the present study was to determine whether some or all of the components of the frontal lobe syndrome are present in children with this disorder. For this purpose, a neuropsychological test battery was administered to 32 unresected epileptic children, aged 8–16 years: 16 with frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE), eight with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) and eight with generalized epilepsy whose principal manifestations were typical absences (GEA). The performances of the three epileptic groups were further compared to normative data derived from 200 French-speaking, healthy children aged 7–16 years, except for standardized tests for which the norms provided in the manual were used. The three epilepsy groups did not differ with respect to conceptual shift and recency memory. However, the FLE children showed deficits in planning and impulse control. Furthermore, they had significantly more coordination problems and exhibited greater rigidity than the other epilepsy groups on the motor tests. These problems were more marked in younger FLE children (8–12 years). The latter were also more impaired on verbal fluency measures. No differences were observed with respect to gender, localization of the epileptic abnormality (unilateral versus bilateral) or medication (monotherapy versus polytherapy). The findings reveal similarities between the neuropsychological profiles of FLE children and adults with frontal lobe lesions.

Introduction

Experimental and clinical evidence points to the importance of the frontal lobes, especially the prefrontal areas, in the mediation of executive functions and motor coordination [8], [27], [65], [67]. Executive functions represent a cognitive construct which refers to the ability to maintain an appropriate problem-solving set for the attainment of future goals [13], [14], [32], [61], [71]. They include planning, self-monitoring, organized search, concept formation, attention and impulse control [72] as well as working memory [7], [52], [53].

Data documenting the role of prefrontal cortex in the organization of executive functions have been derived from a variety of sources such as studies in adult patients with frontal lobe damage [2], [4], [8], [34], [61], [65], lesion studies in non-human primates [11], [13], [19], single-cell recordings [16] and, more recently, neuroradiological studies using regional cerebral blood flow [23], positron emission tomography (PET) [52], [53], single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) [57] and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in patients and healthy subjects [7], [30], [55]. The common finding is that the prefrontal areas are involved in various aspects of executive functions. Furthermore, cerebral imaging procedures have shown that the prefrontal cortex is functionally heterogeneous. For instance, there is evidence that the mid-dorsolateral frontal cortex is associated with working memory whereas the posterior dorsolateral region is crucial for visual associative learning [52], [53]. With regard to motor functions, results of fMRI studies [27] in normal subjects point to the important role of the dorsolateral and medial premotor regions in the programming of trains of repetitive tapping movements.

Most of the research in patients with frontal lobe epilepsy has been carried out on adults who have undergone frontal lobectomy [22], [39], [41], [43]. These patients display deficits on spatial, non-spatial and sensory delay tasks, sorting tasks, tests requiring the monitoring of sequences of external events [15], [29], [41], [42], [43], concept formation and response inhibition tasks [40], [63], associative learning tasks [51], [54], as well as on a variety of attention and memory tasks [25], [63], [67]. In a recent neuropsychological study, Helmstaedter et al. [22] compared the performance of unresected temporal lobe epilepsy and frontal lobe epilepsy adults. The latter performed more poorly on Luria's unimanual and bimanual motor sequencing tasks. They were also inferior to patients with temporal lobe epilepsy with regard to response maintenance and response inhibition on the Stroop Test. Furthermore, they were impaired on tests designed to assess visuo-perceptual speed (rapid discrimination of symbols) and interference (reverse reading of a letter sequence). Finally, they showed impairments with respect to concept formation, planning behaviour and response inhibition on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) and on a Maze test which were similar to those previously observed in patients with various types of frontal lobe lesions [32], [33]. In contrast, the two epilepsy groups did not differ on the Verbal Fluency Test and on a letter cancellation test measuring sustained visual attention.

Studies in children have to take into account developmental aspects of frontal lobe functioning [10]. There is evidence that executive functions emerge in the first year of life, begin to mature between the ages of 4 and 7 years [47], undergo major changes between 8 and 12 years [5], [47] and continue to develop in certain aspects at least until puberty (12–15 years and beyond) [18], [72]. The maturational stages are demonstrable in the children's increasing proficiency on various neuropsychological measures. This is exemplified in a study by Levin et al. [28]. These authors used the WCST, the Verbal Fluency and Graphic Fluency Test, the Tower of London, a Delayed Alternation task and a go–no-go task in healthy children who were divided into three groups along age lines. Children aged 7–8 and 9–12 years demonstrated increasing skills in concept formation and conceptual shift on the WCST and the go–no go task, while the 13- to 14-year-old children showed important gains in the areas of response generation, planning ability and performance speed, as evidenced by an enhanced performance on the Graphic Fluency Test and the Tower of London.

A developmental trend was also observed by Chelune and Baer [5] who found that skills in concept formation, planning and problem-solving, measured on the WCST, developed progressively in children between 6 and 10 years. Around the age of 11 years most of the children were performing the task at adult level.

In another study, conducted by Welsh et al. [72] healthy children aged 3–12 years were submitted to a battery of ‘frontal’ tests in order to determine the point in development at which adult-level performance was achieved. Planning ability, assessed on a simple version of the Tower of Hanoi (TOH), was efficient at the age of 6 years. Children between 3 and 4 years of age showed significant steps towards achievement of this task. Conceptual shift was mastered around the age of 10 years, whereas the development of motor sequencing, verbal search and more complex planning skills (involving the manipulation of four or more disks on the TOH) appear to continue beyond the age of 12, since the 12-year-olds in the study were unable to attain adult level performance in these functions. Evidently, any insult at a given stage in development would be expected to interfere with the emergence of age-appropriate executive functions and motor skills.

Studies in pediatric patient populations have shown that impairments in executive functions can result from a variety of neurological conditions such as closed head injury [29], [30], [37], meningitis [68], brain hemorrhage [20] and exposure to environmental toxins [44], to name but a few. Some of these deficits can be directly related to frontal lobe dysfunction [29], [30], [37]. For instance, using fMRI in children with traumatic head injury, Levin et al [30] observed that frontal lobe damage, but not extrafrontal damage, was associated with impaired performance on the Tower of London.

Distinct patterns of frontal lobe dysfunctions have also been described in four frontally-injured children, studied by Mateer and Williams [38]. The children showed cognitive and behavioural changes marked by impaired attention, academic deficits, irritability and social problems in the absence of apparent intellectual, linguistic or perceptual problems. Two children exhibited difficulties in planning and organization on the Mazes and the Block Design subtests of the revised Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-R). A reduction in verbal fluency (animal names) was observed in another case. Furthermore, all four children showed reduced speed on the interference part of the Stroop Test.

Similar impairments have been reported by Grattan and Esslinger [20] for a patient who suffered a subarachnoid hemorrhage in the left frontal region at the age of 7 years. A 26-year follow-up of this patient further indicated important long-term deficits in psychosocial development and adaptive behaviour which could be attributed to poor auto-regulation. Deficits in self-regulatory and self-monitoring control also dominated the neurobehavioural tableau of a young boy, described by Marlowe [37] who sustained traumatic injury to the right prefrontal cortex. The patient was unable to elaborate and maintain efficient problem-solving strategies in spite of normally developed, even superior, verbal and non-verbal reasoning abilities.

Studies investigating executive functions in children with frontal lobe epilepsy are still scarce. Boone et al. [3], in a single case study, reported a frontal lobe syndrome in conjunction with a focus in the left frontal lobe in a 13-year-old girl. Neuropsychological testing revealed impaired performance on tasks measuring motor speed (Finger Tapping Test), attention and concentration (Digit Span of the WISC-R), alternation between two concepts (Trail Making Test, part B), planning ability (Mazes of the WISC-R) and response inhibition (Stroop Test). The motor performance on the Finger Tapping Test was depressed, particularly for the non-dominant left hand. No deficits were observed on tasks requiring categorization, abstract reasoning and concept formation (WCST). Similarly, Jambaqué and Dulac [24] observed frontal lobe deficits in an 8-year-old boy of normal intelligence who presented with an epileptic focus in the right fronto-temporal region. The child showed marked behavioural and affective changes which abated when adequate seizure control was achieved. On neuropsychological tests, he manifested deficits in motor speed and planning ability on the WISC-R subtests Coding and Mazes. Manual dexterity was also affected as evidenced by a deterioration of his handwriting. Furthermore, he had difficulties reproducing a sequence of hand movements. His verbal fluency was reduced as was his attention span.

To our knowledge, no group studies have been conducted to date that have attempted to distinguish children with frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) from children with other types of epilepsy. The purpose of the present study was to determine whether some or all characteristics of frontal lobe dysfunction are present in children with FLE as compared to children with temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE) or generalized epilepsy (GEA). Based on the few single case studies reviewed above, we expected that the FLE children would do more poorly on measures of motor coordination, mental flexibility, response initiation, impulse control and planning than the TLE and GEA children. We were also interested to know which of the tests employed to assess these functions would be most sensitive to frontal lobe dysfunction in children. The performance of the epileptic children was further compared to normative data derived from healthy children. Finally, the effects of the patients’ age and the localization of the epileptic abnormality (unilateral versus bilateral) were analyzed in order to assess maturation factors and possible effects of the extent of the pathology.

Section snippets

Subjects

Epileptic children. Thirty-two unresected epileptic children aged 8–16 years, participated in this study: 16 patients with frontal lobe epilepsy (12 boys and four girls) and 16 children (eight boys and eight girls) with other epilepsy types. The latter included eight patients with temporal lobe epilepsy (four boys and four girls) and eight with generalized epilepsy (four boys and four girls) whose main clinical manifestations were typical absence seizures. Eight of the FLE patients and all TLE

Preliminary tests

The demographic variables of the three epilepsy groups (see Table 1) were submitted to separate analyses of variance (ANOVA) to determine whether the groups were adequately matched. Furthermore, a Chi-square analysis was performed comparing the number of FLE and TLE children on monotherapy with those on polytherapy.

Experimental tests

A multivariate analysis (MANOVA) with group as a factor and age in years as covariate were performed on the scores of six of the seven experimental tests (tests 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7)

Preliminary tests

Inspection of Table 1 reveals that there was a higher proportion of boys in the FLE group. The ANOVA revealed that the three epilepsy groups did not differ with respect to other demographic or clinical variables such as age at testing (F(2,29)=0.724, P>0.05), age at seizure onset (F(2,29)=0.412, P>0.05) and duration of the epilepsy (F=(2,29)=0.270, P>0.05). Similarly, the Chi-square analysis comparing the number of FLE and TLE children on monotherapy and polytherapy was not significant (χ2

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether children suffering from frontal lobe epilepsy would present some or all of the characteristics of frontal lobe dysfunction seen in adult patients with frontal lobe lesions. Furthermore, in addition to providing a neuropsychological profile of children with FLE, the results of the present study were also expected to provide information as to which of the tests used in the assessment of frontal functions would be most useful in

Conclusion

A specific neuropsychological profile was obtained for FLE children that distinguished them from children with TLE and GEA on several measures that purport to assess frontal lobe functions. FLE children exhibited many characteristics that are commonly associated with frontal lobe dysfunction in adult patients. More specifically, they showed impairments in motor coordination, response generation, impulse control and planning ability. Motor deficits were more marked for tasks involving bimanual

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by grants from the Agence Canadienne de Développement International (ACDI) and the Québec Formation de Chercheurs et Aide à la Recherche (FCAR) awarded to Maria Teresa Hernandez and Maryse Lassonde, respectively. We wish to thank Dr Gabriel Leonard for his valuable comments.

References (74)

  • M. Petrides et al.

    Deficits on subject-ordered tasks after frontal and temporal lobe lesions in man

    Neuropsychologia

    (1982)
  • M.L. Smith et al.

    Estimation of frequency of occurrence of abstract designs after frontal or temporal lobectomy

    Neuropsychologia

    (1988)
  • C. Addy

    Cognitive functions in children with epilepsy

    Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology

    (1987)
  • Bancaud J, Talairach J. Clinical semiology of frontal lobe seizures. In: Chauvel P, Delgado-Escueta AV, Halgren E,...
  • K.B. Boone et al.

    Neuropsychological and behavioral abnormalities in an adolescent with frontal lobe seizures

    Neurology

    (1988)
  • Botez MI. Les syndromes du lobe frontal In: Botez MI, editor. Neuropsychologie Cinique et Neurologie du Comportement....
  • G.J. Chelune et al.

    Developmental norms for the Wisconsin card sorting test

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (1986)
  • G.J. Chelune et al.

    Evaluation of the general sensitivity of the Wisconsin card sorting test among younger and older children

    Developmental Neuropyschology

    (1987)
  • J. Cohen et al.

    Activation of prefrontal cortex in a nonspatial working memory task with functional MRI

    Human Brain Mapping

    (1993)
  • Dames AR, Anderson SW. The frontal lobes In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E, editors. Clinical Neuropsychology. New York:...
  • A.V. Delgado-Escueta et al.

    Frontal lobe seizures and epilepsies in neurobehavioral disorders

    Advances in Neurology

    (1991)
  • Dempster FN. Resistance to interference: developmental changes in a basic processing mechanism In: Pasnak R, Howe ML,...
  • Diamond A. Differences between adult and infant cognition: Is the crucial variable presence or absence of language? In:...
  • Dubois B, Pillon B, Sirigu A. Fonctions intégratrices et cortex préfrontal chez l'homme. In: Seron X, Jeannerod M,...
  • J. Duncan

    Disorganization of behavior after frontal lobe damage

    Cognitive Neuropsychology

    (1986)
  • Fuster JM. Single-unit studies of prefrontal cortex. In: Perecman E, editor. The Frontal Lobes Revisited. New York:...
  • Goldberg E, Bilder RM. The frontal lobes and hierarchical organization of cognitive control. In: Perecman E, editor....
  • Golden CJ. The Luria-Nebraska Children's Battery: Theory and formulation. In: Hynd GW, Obrzut JE, editors....
  • Goldman-Rakic PS, Isseroff A, Schwartz M, Bugbee N. The neurobiology of cognitive development. In: Haith MM, Campos J,...
  • Heaton RK. A Manual for the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources,...
  • D.H. Ingvar

    Memory of the future: An essay on the temporal organisation of conscious awareness

    Human Neurobiology

    (1985)
  • I. Jambaqué et al.

    Syndrome frontal réversible et épilepsie chez un enfant de 8 ans

    Archives Françaises de Pédiatrie

    (1989)
  • Kemper B, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Kognitive Profile von praechirurgischen Patienten mit Frontal- und...
  • H.S. Levin et al.

    Developmental changes in performance on test of purported frontal lobe functioning

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1991)
  • H.S. Levin et al.

    Dimension of cognition measured by the Tower of London and other cognitive tasks in head-injured children and adolescents

    Developmental Neuropsychology

    (1996)
  • H.S. Levin et al.

    Tower of London performance in relation to magnetic resonance imaging following closed head injury in children

    Neuropsychology

    (1994)
  • Lezak MD. Neuropsychological Assessment. New York: Oxford University Press,...
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text