Elsevier

Neuropsychologia

Volume 38, Issue 13, December 2000, Pages 1734-1740
Neuropsychologia

Emotional curiosity: modulation of visuospatial attention by arousal is preserved in aging and early-stage Alzheimer’s disease

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00077-4Get rights and content

Abstract

Previous studies have shown that Alzheimer’s disease, even in its early stages, decreases novelty-seeking behaviors (curiosity) and impairs the shifting of spatial attention to extrapersonal targets. In this study, early-stage probable Alzheimer’s disease patients (PRAD) and young and aging controls were shown pairs of visual scenes, some of which contained emotionally-arousing material, while eye movements were recorded under free viewing conditions. In all three subject groups, emotionally-arousing scenes attracted more viewing time and also became the preferential target of the initial visual orientation. Our findings suggest that the arousing properties of sensory stimuli may overcome some of the AD-related impairments in the distribution of attention to extrapersonal targets. These results may have implications for interventions aimed at improving the cognitive symptoms of PRAD.

Introduction

The neuropsychological hallmark of Alzheimer’s disease (AD) is a severe impairment of memory function. However, the adaptive deployment of attention to extrapersonal events is also frequently disrupted early in the course of the disease and interferes with activities of daily living. These attentional deficits are multifaceted and include altered visual search strategies [8], [22], [24], an inability to disengage the focus of attention [20], [23] and reduced orientation to novel and complex stimuli [4]. Furthermore, while AD patients may be able to focus attention globally or locally when each mode is tested in a separate experiment, they have difficulty when required to switch between these processing modes in the same block of experimental trials [7].

One possible strategy for improving attentional engagement in AD is to enhance the emotional impact of environmental stimuli. Several studies have argued for the preservation of emotional processing relative to the cognitive decline in mild AD [1], [2], [12], [25]. Thus, it might be predicted that arousal provoked by emotionally-charged stimuli would deploy attentional resources toward the pertinent sectors of extrapersonal space. The influence of emotional arousal on the regulation of attention in AD has not been directly investigated and is the focus of the current study.

Section snippets

Subjects

A total of 42 subjects participated in this study. Nine patients with mild probable Alzheimer’s disease (PRAD) and nine nondemented aging controls (AC) were recruited from the Clinical Core subject registry of the Northwestern Alzheimer’s Disease Center. Subjects had undergone neurological and neuropsychological examinations to establish the diagnosis of PRAD in the patient group [21] and the absence of cognitive deficits or alterations in daily living activities in the control group. In order

Experiment 2

The modulation of visuospatial attention in Experiment 1 is likely related to the arousal properties of the stimuli. It is possible, however, that lower-level perceptual features contributed to the observed oculographic patterns. For example, mere attention to stimulus color could account for the results if the subjects were differentially attending to color information across the stimulus categories. This is important to consider for negatively-valent scenes, as some of them contain images of

General discussion

The results of Experiment 1 demonstrate that, in the early stages of AD, patients can direct their attention to environmental stimuli in a normal manner when the stimuli contain negatively-valent emotional content. ‘Emotional curiosity’ as measured within our paradigm contains two components: an early attentional orienting component that directs the first saccade toward the emotionally-arousing cue in the pair and a sustained attention component that keeps the eyes focused on the emotional

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Shelly Huddle for assistance with data collection and analysis. Supported in part by a NARSAD Young Investigator Award (KSL), NIH Grant NS30863-03 (MMM) and NIA Grant AG14068 (SW).

References (31)

  • F. Esteves et al.

    Automatically elicited fear: Conditioned skin conductance responses to masked facial expressions

    Cognition and Emotion

    (1994)
  • G.G. Fillenbaum et al.

    Comparison of two screening tests in Alzheimer’s disease: The correlation and reliability of the Mini-Mental State Examination and the Modified Blessed Test

    Archives of Neurology

    (1987)
  • J.V. Filoteo et al.

    Directed and divided attention in Alzheimer’s disease: Impairment in shifting of attention to global and local stimuli

    Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology

    (1992)
  • N.S. Foldi et al.

    Selective attention skills in Alzheimer’s disease: Performance on graded cancellation tests varying in density and complexity

    Journal of Gerontology

    (1992)
  • M. Gallagher et al.

    Functions of the amygdala and related forebrain areas in attention and cognition

    Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences USA

    (1999)
  • Cited by (76)

    • Impact of emotionally negative information on attentional processes in normal aging and Alzheimer's disease

      2020, Brain and Cognition
      Citation Excerpt :

      To this end, we used an eye-tracking paradigm and a visual search task that involved emotional and neutral targets. On the basis of studies in AD that suggested (a) impairment of early engagement toward emotional stimuli in AD (Bourgin et al., 2018), (b) preservation of the impact of emotional information on delayed attentional processes in AD (Berger et al., 2015; Doninger and Bylsma, 2007; LaBar et al., 2000; Monti et al., 2010), and (c) early alterations of the amygdala, which seems preferentially involved in engagement processes, we expected to observe selective impairment of engagement mechanisms toward emotional information in patients with AD (relative to HOAs and YAs). Regarding normal aging, some studies suggest that HOAs are more efficient than YAs at inhibiting negative information (Hahn et al., 2006; Mather and Carstensen, 2003).

    • The Social Regulation of Emotion: An Integrative, Cross-Disciplinary Model

      2016, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Under such circumstances, the identification stage is altered for the regulator because they are responding to an anticipated emotional reaction by the target rather than to an existing one. Emotional elicitors draw attention [78,79] and regulators can modify a target's emotions by shifting their attention away from undesirable elicitors and towards others. Returning to the example of the man arguing with his friend at a social gathering, his wife might draw his attention to someone else in the room and reinforce that redeployment of attention by focusing her attention on them as well.

    • Selective looking at natural scenes: Hedonic content and gender

      2015, International Journal of Psychophysiology
    • Interactions between Attention and Emotion

      2015, Brain Mapping: An Encyclopedic Reference
    • Aging is associated with changes in the neural circuits underlying empathy

      2014, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      Behavioral, anatomic, and neuroimaging evidences support that emotion processing is relatively well preserved in healthy older adults despite changes in other cognitive domains (for a review, see Reuter-Lorenz and Lustig, 2005). For instance, compared with young adults, older adults are just as likely to understand emotional stimuli (Keightley et al., 2006; LaBar et al., 2000; Mather and Knight, 2006), and report intense emotional experience (Levine and Bluck, 1997). Additionally, older adults have greater emotional regulation to view emotional stimuli (Carstensen et al., 2011; Gross et al., 1997).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text