Reasons for disclosing depression matter: The consequences of having egosystem and ecosystem goals

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2008.03.016Get rights and content

Abstract

People with depression often conceal their depression and do not seek help, in part because of the stigma associated with mental illness. We propose two motivational systems for the self: egosystem motivations, concerned with protecting and inflating desired self-images, and ecosystem motivations, concerned with contributing or supporting others. Using a sample of 48 individuals at a Midwestern university, USA, this study examined motivations for disclosing or concealing a concealable stigma, to test the hypotheses that: (1) these motivations load on two factors corresponding to egosystem and ecosystem goals, and (2) ecosystem motivations for disclosure have more positive effects on disclosure and psychological well-being. We found that people with egosystem goals disclosed less, and also experienced lower psychological well-being when they did disclose. Conversely, people with ecosystem goals disclosed more and experienced greater psychological well-being while doing so. Implications of these findings for deciding when and to whom to disclose one's depression are discussed.

Introduction

People with concealable stigmas must decide when to disclose their stigmatizing condition (Goffman, 1963, Jones et al., 1984). A great deal is at stake, both for the person who could be stigmatized and for others. People who disclose a stigma risk the possibility of being stereotyped, rejected, discriminated against, and even the target of hate crimes (Goffman, 1963, Kelly and McKillop, 1996). When possible, people may choose to conceal stigmatizing characteristics (Jones et al., 1984, Link et al., 1991) to avoid potentially awkward interactions with non-stigmatized others (Goffman, 1963). Fearing embarrassment and negative judgment, some people decide to not seek professional help for their depression (Barney, Griffiths, Jorm, & Christensen, 2006). Thus, stigma can prevent people from disclosing their depression to health care providers – potentially their greatest source of support.

Simultaneously, disclosing a stigma can help both the self and others. Disclosing enables stigmatized people to both receive support and provide social support to others who have the stigma, thereby decreasing feelings of loneliness and isolation for the self and others. According to a recent review, contact between non-stigmatized and stigmatized group members leads to a reduction in stereotyping, prejudice, and stigma (Rusch, Angermeyer, & Corrigan, 2005). Because one's mental health status is not always readily visible, disclosure may be necessary to facilitate contact. Thus, disclosing to non-stigmatized others may reduce stigma.

Given the costs and benefits of revealing one's depression, why do people conceal or disclose? When do people determine that disclosing is too risky? When do they risk rejection to obtain needed support? We attempt to answer these questions by investigating motivations for disclosure.

In this article, we draw on our recent research and theorizing about motivational orientations for the self to identify factors that predict disclosure and disclosure's impact on well-being. Specifically, we propose that egosystem motivations undermine well-being, inhibit disclosure, and make disclosure a less positive experience. Ecosystem motivations, in contrast, foster well-being, encourage disclosure, and make disclosure a more positive experience.

We have proposed two distinct motivational orientations for the self, the egosystem and the ecosystem (Crocker, Garcia, & Nuer, 2008; Crocker, Nuer, Olivier, & Cohen, 2006). Virtually, any action can stem from either of these motivational frameworks, or a blend of the two. People have relatively stable average levels of egosystem and ecosystem motivations, but situations (e.g., domain, interaction partner, situational cues) cause variations in those average levels (Crocker & Canevello, in press).

In the egosystem framework, the direction of motivation is toward the self; people focus on satisfying their own desires and needs, giving them priority over the well-being of others. Constructing and maintaining desired self-images, obtaining social resources, and avoiding social harm comprise central egosystem goals (Leary and Kowalski, 1990, Miller, 2006, Pyszczynski et al., 2004). Constructing desired images requires doing things to ensure that other people see and acknowledge those qualities in the self (Leary & Kowalski, 1990). Research and theory suggest a core human motivation to construct, maintain, and defend positive images of the self and to see the self as worthy and valuable (e.g., Greenberg et al., 1993, Leary and Baumeister, 2000, Steele, 1988).

Drawing on the biological notion of an ecosystem, Crocker et al. (2006) used the term “ecosystem motivation” to refer to a motivational framework in which people see themselves as part of a larger whole; a system of individuals whose needs are equally important, and whose actions have consequences for others, with repercussions for the entire system, ultimately affecting the ability of the individual to satisfy his or her own fundamental needs. The direction of motivation is toward others: people consider others' needs and well-being, and prioritize them along with the well-being of the self. In this framework, people do not act at the expense of the self, but rather these goals are more like a form of enlightened self-interest (Batson, 1998). Consequently, people with ecosystem motivations prioritize the needs of others, not out of virtue or self-sacrifice but rather to satisfy their own and others' fundamental needs.

We suggest that egosystem and ecosystem motivations help explain when people disclose and have a better experience doing so. Specifically, we propose that these motivations will influence both psychological well-being and disclosure.

Ecosystem motivations may help depressed people break out of their negative, ruminative self-focus, thereby increasing their well-being. People with depression tend to experience a heightened self-focus state (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991, Pyszczynski and Greenberg, 1987). When people have a heightened self-focus, they perpetuate depressed mood states by focusing exclusively on themselves and not considering their external environment (Pyszczynski & Greenberg, 1987), which detracts from effective problem solving strategies (Nolen-Hoeksema, 1991). Thus, egosystem motivations, which primarily focus on costs and benefits to the self, may be particularly prevalent for people with depression, and be associated with lower psychological well-being. Conversely, ecosystem motivations, which focus on the needs of both the self and others, may help people terminate ruminative self-focus and lead to greater psychological well-being.

In fact, recent research shows that egosystem and ecosystem goals affect well-being, and they do so more strongly for dysphoric than nondysphoric people. In one study, participants completed brief questionnaires about their current activities, their reasons for doing the activity, and their well-being approximately four times a day for 2 weeks (Crocker, Breines, Luhtanen, & Denning, 2006, Study 1). Results revealed that participants experienced lower well-being when they completed their current activity because it had value for them (egosystem motivation), but higher well-being when they did their current activity because it had value for others (ecosystem motivation). These findings were stronger for depressed participants. Similarly, in a study of incoming college freshmen, students with chronic ecosystem goals (averaged across 10 weekly reports) showed decreased symptoms of depression from the beginning to the end of their first semester of college, whereas students with chronic egosystem goals showed increases in symptoms of depression (Crocker, Breines, & Flynn, in preparation). These two studies provide evidence that egosystem goals related to lower well-being and ecosystem goals correlated with improved well-being, especially for people who feel relatively depressed.

When people fear disapproval, they only disclose information that is not very intimate (Meleshko & Alden, 1993). We propose that fear of disapproval or rejection leads people to conceal a stigma if they have egosystem goals, but not if they have ecosystem goals. When people have egosystem goals, they want to maintain and enhance self-images and obtain desired social outcomes such as acceptance or admiration. Consequently, when stigmatized people expect rejection if they reveal their stigma, they will attempt to conceal it from others.

Ecosystem goals for disclosing stigmatized identities, such as depression, include consideration of the needs and well-being of others, in addition to the self. Instead of focusing on inflating or protecting the self, ecosystem goals focus on supporting others or contributing. People with ecosystem motivations might be willing to risk disapproval for the sake of creating authentic relationships with, educating, or supporting others. Consequently, when they have ecosystem goals, stigmatized people may disclose their stigma despite expecting rejection, or even because they expect rejection and consequently want to educate others about their stigma. Thus, we predict that people disclose their stigma more often when they have ecosystem goals and less when they have egosystem goals, and the effect of these goals on disclosure should be stronger when people expect rejection.

We used experience sampling methodology (Hektner, Schmidt, & Csikszentmihaly, 2007) to examine the relationships between egosystem and ecosystem motivations and psychological well-being and disclosure. Experience sampling methodology allows researchers to examine participants in their natural setting over a sustained period of time. We asked participants to indicate their motivations to disclose their concealable stigma over 2 weeks, which enabled us to examine both the immediate and long-term effects of having egosystem and ecosystem motivations to disclose.

Two stigmatized groups participated: people with depression and sexual minorities. The inclusion of sexual minorities in this study allows us to ascertain if the effects of egosystem and ecosystem motivations on disclosure and well-being apply to other concealable stigmas or are unique to people with depression. Thus, sexual minority participants serve as a comparison group in this study.

Section snippets

Participants

Fifty people who self-identified as gay, lesbian, or bisexual, and 48 people who self-identified as depressed participated in this study, which was conducted at a large Midwestern research university. Three people in the sexual minority group and three people in the depressed group did not complete time 2 questionnaires. We excluded them from analyses using time 2 results. The sexual minority group included 27 females and 20 males. The ethnic breakdown of this group was 72% Caucasians, 8.5%

Analysis plan

First, we conducted a factor analysis to examine if reasons for and against disclosure and goals scales loaded on the two hypothesized theoretical factors: egosystem and ecosystem goals; we then tested whether the goals differed by group. We next conducted a series of regression analyses to examine the effects of these goals on disclosure frequencies and psychological well-being both over time and in daily disclosure decisions. Finally, we examined possible reasons for group differences in

Discussion

In an experience sampling study, we explored the consequences of egosystem and ecosystem motivations for disclosing a concealable stigma. The results of this study suggest that people do have egosystem and ecosystem motivations for disclosure and these goals provide a useful framework for understanding when people disclose a stigma and how they feel when they do so. We will discuss the effects of goals and perceptions of stigma on disclosure and their implications in turn.

Conclusions

People with concealable stigmas must continually decide whether to reveal their stigma to others, or try to conceal it. For people with depression, disclosing one's depression is necessary to obtain needed support. Our results indicate that in daily disclosure decisions, people feel better and disclose more often when they consider the needs of others (i.e., have ecosystem motivations). However, especially for depressed participants, our findings suggest that it is also important to consider

Acknowledgements

Julie A. Garcia was supported by a Ford Dissertation Fellowship and a National Science Foundation Minority Postdoctoral fellowship, and Jennifer Crocker was supported by National Institute of Mental Health grant R01 MH58869 during the preparation of this manuscript. A dissertation grant from the University of Michigan awarded to the first author and a National Institute of Mental Health grant awarded to the second author funded the present research.

References (34)

  • S.W. Cole et al.

    Elevated physical health risk among gay men who conceal their homosexual identity

    Health Psychology

    (1996)
  • Crocker, J., Breines, J. G., & Flynn, H. Relationship goals and changes in depressive symptoms in first-semester...
  • Crocker, J., Breines, J. G., Luhtanen, R.K., & Denning, P. (2006). From egosystem to ecosystem: Implications for...
  • Crocker, J., & Canevello, A. (in press). Creating and undermining social support: the role of compassionate and...
  • J. Crocker et al.

    From egosystem to ecosystem in intergroup interactions: implicaitons for intergroup reconciliation

  • V.J. Derlega et al.

    Reasons for and against disclosing HIV-seropositive test results to an intimate partner: a functional perspective

  • B.M. Dykman

    Integrating cognitive and motivational factors in depression: initial tests of a goal orientation approach

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1998)
  • Cited by (53)

    • Disclosure processes as predictors of relationship outcomes among people in recovery from opioid use disorder: A longitudinal analysis

      2021, Drug and Alcohol Dependence
      Citation Excerpt :

      Disclosure goals may impact whether individuals disclose as well as act as a lens through which individuals perceive and engage in disclosures. Some work suggests that disclosure goals are associated with likelihood of disclosure: Approach goals are generally associated with greater likelihood of disclosure whereas avoidance goals are generally associated with lower likelihood of disclosure (Derlega et al., 2002; Garcia and Crocker, 2008). Once people decide to disclose, the Disclosure Process Model theorizes that people with a greater focus on approach goals attend more to the presence of positive stimuli within disclosures and interpret neutral or ambiguous stimuli positively, whereas people with a greater focus on avoidance goals attend more to the presence of negative stimuli and interpret neutral or ambiguous stimuli negatively (Chaudoir and Fisher, 2010).

    • Impacts of the COVID-19 pandemic on health, financial worries, and perceived organizational support among people living with disabilities in Canada

      2021, Disability and Health Journal
      Citation Excerpt :

      This was especially true for those living with both a physical and mental health disability. Issues of workplace disclosure of a disability and accommodation needs are receiving increased attention in disability research.21,22,32–37 They highlight that challenges encountered by people living with a disability go beyond the symptoms and limitations created by health conditions, and revolve around perceptions of support, concerns about reprisals if a disability is disclosed, and other psychosocial work factors.21,35,38

    • CEO humility, narcissism and firm innovation: A paradox perspective on CEO traits

      2017, Leadership Quarterly
      Citation Excerpt :

      In contrast, inflated self-views cause narcissists to desire personal recognition and glory (Maccoby, 2004), power and prestige (Raskin & Hall, 1981), and continuous reaffirmation of superiority (Rosenthal & Pittinsky, 2006), all components of egosystem motivation, an orientation toward protecting or inflating the self-image and treating others as “agents of validation or invalidation of the self” (Crocker et al., 2008, p. 173). While most people may be stronger in one orientation than the other, some are driven by a blend of both ecosystem and egosystem motivations (Garcia & Crocker, 2008). For example, negotiators who use win-win strategies may be driven by both self-concern and other-orientation (Nauta, De Dreu, & Van Der Vaart, 2002).

    • Disclosure of a communication disorder during a job interview: A theoretical model

      2020, Journal of Communication Disorders
      Citation Excerpt :

      Those with spasmodic dysphonia have reported speaking voices that are both weakened and effortful (Smith et al., 1998), with some individuals experiencing breathlessness and fatigue in occupations that require extended voice use (Baylor, Yorkston, & Eadie, 2005). Fatigue has been implicated as a major barrier to workplace integration across a number of different communication disorders (Garcia & Crocker, 2008). Therefore, how relevant symptoms might be for a particular job could factor into the decision of whether or not to disclose during the interview.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text