Elsevier

NeuroImage

Volume 36, Issue 3, 1 July 2007, Pages 522-531
NeuroImage

Functional neural dynamics underlying auditory event-related N1 and N1 suppression response

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.027Get rights and content

Abstract

Presenting tone triplets of identical stimuli preceded by silent intervals of 30 s produces a series of three N1 averaged event-related potentials (ERPs), the first being of greater amplitude (non-suppressed N1) than the second and third ones (suppressed N1). Maximal statistically independent components (ICs) of single-trial multi-electrode scalp EEG responses to triplets were obtained by ICA algorithm, and then each IC was searched for underlying brain structures by LORETA inverse solution, and for oscillatory contributions by time–frequency analysis. Non-suppressed N1 cortical mechanisms were broken down into five ICs, grouped in two time-windows (early-onset and late-onset) involving the participation of temporal, frontal and parietal structures, and sub-serving EEG oscillatory contributions of power enhancement and putative phase concentration of mainly theta, alpha and low beta bands. Suppressed N1 was due to the modulation of two above-mentioned early-onset ICs, involving temporal structures only, and mainly sub-serving oscillatory contributions of phase concentration of theta and alpha. Present results, showing quantifiable changes of IC descriptors – i.e. time window of activation, implied structures and oscillatory contributions – extracted from two distinct brain functional situations (non-suppressed versus suppressed N1), give support to the view that ICA is not merely a statistical “latent variables” model when applied to ERPs, but could help to capture underlying specific function subunits of brain dynamics.

Introduction

Brain electrical responses time-locked to events (stimulus, response or behavioural task) have been studied for decades by averaging the event-locked single trials to obtain event-related potentials (ERPs). Although averaged ERPs resemble basic waves, they undergo complex neural dynamic activity that is likely to arise from multiple temporarily and spatially distributed specialized brain sources (Bressler and Kelso, 2001). Hence, it has been suggested that large-scale interaction of these neural contributions to ERPs gives rise to their functional integration, which might lead to support of specific function activities (Friston, 2005). In addition, the relevance of oscillatory activity as a framework for the functional integration of a distributed neural activity has been pointed out (Fries, 2005). Indeed, converging recent evidence showed that a distributed functionally conformed network of neurons might be transiently linked by reciprocal dynamic connections (Varela et al., 2001), thus resulting in multiple feedback loops that give rise to oscillatory neural-based behaviour (Bressler, 2002). Therefore, based on these arguments, underlying neural functional properties given by an ERP are unlikely to be captured solely by studying the representation of the temporal course and scalp features of the averaged electrical activity, but might require identification of their related temporal evolution, cortical topography and oscillatory behaviour (Fogelson et al., 2006), which can only be captured at the single-trial level.

The concept of ERPs as being composed of orchestrated clusters of distributed neural contributions, which transiently integrate to support specific functional activities, has been recently enlarged on the basis of ICA (Independent Component Analysis) results on multi-electrode EEG data (Makeig et al., 2002, Penny et al., 2002). Accordingly, ERPs could be broken down into maximal statistically independent contributions (independent components, ICs), which might be characterised by specific temporo-structural (Makeig et al., 2002, Marco-Pallarés et al., 2005) and frequency domain features (Makeig et al., 2004a). This leads to the proposition that the properties of maximal statistical independence and the multifaceted physiological characterization (in the time-, space- and frequency-domains) of each IC strongly suggest that they might also pick up specific functional brain subunits. However, at present, there is no direct evidence that neural activity identified by an IC intrinsically modulate to functional experimental manipulations, though this would, unequivocally, transfer meaningful functional entity to the given neural activity.

The first objective of this study was to provide evidence that separate specific functional activity of brain computing captured by ICs and their descriptors (i.e. time–structures–frequency) show quantifiable changes in distinct functional situations. To this end, we selected auditory N1, which in front of identical external repeated stimulation shows reduced amplitude (attenuation or repetitive suppression) (Näätänen, 1992, Friston, 2005). This reduced amplitude is associated with the prevention of the inflow of redundant information from the environment into daily life (Näätänen, 1992). The advantage of this experimental paradigm resides in that, as the stimuli of the sequence are physically identical, functional changes sub-serving N1 amplitude reduction other than the ones experienced by its intrinsic neural substrate can be discarded.

The second objective of this work is to advance on the characterization of functional neural dynamics underlying auditory event-related N1 and N1 suppression response. Auditory N1 ERP involves a complex neural network of several dynamic properties described by temporal, spatial and spectral contributions. Hence, in the time domain, scalp electromagnetic (MEG) (Loveless et al., 1996, McEvoy et al., 1997) studies showed distinct sequential N1 contribution activations, referred to as “early” (∼ 80 ms) and “late” N1 (∼ 130 ms) (Näätänen, 1992). Further, in the spatial domain, N1 are sub-served by a multi-generator process (Naatanen and Picton, 1987), involving temporal (Hari et al., 1980), frontal (Knight et al., 1980), cingulate (Tzourio et al., 1997) and parietal regions (Knight et al., 1980). And, in the frequency domain, the study of the underlying auditory N1 neural oscillatory activity showed the involvement of a multi-frequency phenomenon enclosing enhanced spectral power and phase-resetting of theta and alpha frequency bands at scalp EEG (Jansen et al., 2003, Fuentemilla et al., 2006). In addition, it has been argued that suppressed N1 might be due to the functional modulation of basic N1 processes, such as distinct EEG recovery cycles of N1 “early” and “late” contributions (Loveless et al., 1996), to decreased auditory cortical neuron activity (Rosburg et al., 2006) or to a reduction in scalp theta and alpha phase synchrony together with a disappearance of a concomitant spectral power modulation (Fuentemilla et al., 2006). However, to date, no integrated approach has made a comparative study of auditory N1 and N1 suppressed addressing the behaviour of time–structure–frequency IC descriptors relating to N1 and N1 repetitive suppression, though it could provide new insights in the study of N1 functional neural dynamics.

Here, we tackle these objectives by applying a 3-step approach analysis. The first two steps were based on the combined Independent Component Analysis (ICA) (Bell and Sejnowski, 1995, Jung et al., 2001) + Low-Resolution Tomography (LORETA) (Pascual-Marqui et al., 1994, Pascual-Marqui, 1999) proposed by Marco-Pallarés et al. (2005). ICA has been used to “blindly” identify neural modes describing activity in concurrent electromagnetic activity that is spatially fixed and temporally independent (Makeig et al., 1996), while LORETA analysis of the spatial maps associated with each IC provided solutions to the inverse problem of the neural source location of each component (Marco-Pallarés et al., 2005). In addition, the spectral content of each IC will be studied by computing a single-trial time–frequency analysis (Makeig et al., 2004b).

Section snippets

Subjects

16 right-handed healthy subjects (7 female), 25.4 ± 1.6 years (range, 20–28 years), after complete description of the work, gave their written consent to participate in the study. All subjects had no history of head injury, neurological disease, audiological problems, severe medical illness or drug abuse. The experiment complied with the Code of Ethics of the World Medical Association (Declaration of Helsinki) and was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University of Barcelona.

Stimuli and procedure

Stimuli

Event-related potentials

All tones (S1, S2 and S3) elicited significant N1 ERP to all electrode locations studied (P < 0.001). 4 subjects were excluded for the analysis due to technical problems on 4 electrodes (CP1, TP3, CP3 and TP4). Fig. 1 depicts the grand average of ERP waveforms to each stimulus across 12 subjects at Fz, C3, Cz, C4 and Pz electrodes and the scalp topography of the N1 peak amplitude to each stimuli. N1 attenuation was measured by N1 decreased amplitude across each electrode (F(2,22) = 15.16, P < 0.001)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Arnaud Delorme and Scott Makeig for their generous gift of software. This study was supported by grants to CG from the Generalitat de Catalunya to support NECOM group (SGR2005-00831), the Spanish Ministerio de Ciencia y Tecnología (SEJ2006-13998), the European Union (FP6-507231, SENSATION), and the Fundació Marató 2006-061632.

References (54)

  • R.T. Knight et al.

    The effects of frontal and temporal–parietal lesions on the auditory evoked potential in man

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1980)
  • N. Loveless et al.

    Temporal integration in auditory sensory memory: neuromagnetic evidence

    Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1996)
  • S. Makeig

    Auditory event-related dynamics of the EEG spectrum and effects of exposure to tones

    Electroencephal. Clin. Neurophysiol.

    (1993)
  • S. Makeig et al.

    Mining event-related brain dynamics

    Trends Cogn. Sci.

    (2004)
  • J. Marco-Pallarés et al.

    Combined ICA-LORETA analysis of mismatch negativity

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • C. Mulert et al.

    Integration of fMRI and simultaneous EEG: towards a comprehensive understanding of localization and time-course of brain activity in target detection

    NeuroImage

    (2004)
  • R.D. Pascual-Marqui et al.

    Low resolution electromagnetic tomography: a new method for localizing electrical activity in the brain

    Int. J. Psychophysiol.

    (1994)
  • W.D. Penny et al.

    Event-related brain dynamics

    Trends Neurosci.

    (2002)
  • T. Rinne et al.

    Superior temporal and inferior frontal cortices are activated by infrequent sound duration decrements: an fMRI study

    NeuroImage

    (2005)
  • N. Tzourio et al.

    Functional anatomy of human auditory attention studied with PET

    NeuroImage

    (1997)
  • R.G. Andrzejak et al.

    Detecting event-related time-dependent directional couplings

    New J. Phys.

    (2006)
  • J. Ahveninen et al.

    Task-modulated “what” and “where” pathways in human auditory cortex

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2006)
  • M. Alcaini et al.

    Two separate frontal components in the N1 wave of the human auditory evoked response

    Psychophysiology

    (1994)
  • A.J. Bell et al.

    An information-maximization approach to blind separation and blind deconvolution

    Neural Comput.

    (1995)
  • S.L. Bressler

    The Handbook of Brain Theory and Neural Networks

    (2002)
  • R.T. Canolty et al.

    High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex

    Science

    (2006)
  • N. Fogelson et al.

    Different functional loops between cerebral cortex and the subthalmic area in Parkinson’s disease

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2006)
  • Cited by (43)

    • Disrupted auditory N1, theta power and coherence suppression to willed speech in people with schizophrenia

      2023, NeuroImage: Clinical
      Citation Excerpt :

      Cumulatively, these findings were interpreted as evidence that suppression of theta coherence may be a specific biomarker for schizophrenia, as distinct from traditional N1 suppression which may index a more general psychosis vulnerability. The N1 component is a complex amalgam of several sources (Woods, 1995) and contains both spontaneous (or nonphase-locked) power in the theta range, and power from other frequency bands (Grau et al., 2007). In contrast, the theta coherence measure is specific to phase-resetting activity, which is argued to be initiated by the detection of an external sound stimulus and suppressed by the presence of a concurrent motor command to initiate speech (Wang et al., 2014).

    • Early auditory-evoked potentials in body dysmorphic disorder: An ERP/sLORETA study

      2021, Psychiatry Research
      Citation Excerpt :

      Previous studies demonstrated that the auditory N100 is mainly generated in the primary and secondary auditory cortices (Giard et al., 1994; Pantev et al., 1995; Thoma et al., 2003). However, several studies proposed additional sources in the dorsal anterior cingulate cortex, the inferior parietal, ventrolateral prefrontal cortices (Grau et al., 2007), as well as the hippocampus, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, and thalamus (Boutros et al., 2008). Furthermore, these sources partially overlapped with the Posterior Cingulate Cortex (PCC), a central node in the default mode network (DMN: Giard et al., 1994; Pantev et al., 1995).

    • Long-latency event-related responses to vowels: N1-P2 decomposition by two-step principal component analysis

      2020, International Journal of Psychophysiology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Temporospatial factors T1S1 and T1S2 were both extracted from T1 in the second PCA step, and were interpreted as corresponding to Näätänen and Picton's component 3 and component 1, respectively. Component 3 is thought to reflect attention call signals triggered by a transient detection mechanism (Lange, 2012, 2013; Näätänen and Picton, 1987; Sanmiguel et al., 2013), possibly involving nonspecific frontal regions (Atcherson et al., 2006; Grau et al., 2007; Zhang et al., 2011). Hence, the corresponding temporospatial factor T1S1 would be expected to be highly sensitive to attention catching properties of stimuli, but not to be affected by vowel quality.

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text