Elsevier

Neurobiology of Aging

Volume 60, December 2017, Pages 153-163
Neurobiology of Aging

Regular article
Dynamic range in BOLD modulation: lifespan aging trajectories and association with performance

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2017.08.027Get rights and content

Abstract

Alteration of dynamic range of modulation to cognitive difficulty has been proposed as a salient predictor of cognitive aging. Here, we examine in 171 adults (aged 20–94 years) the effects of age on dynamic modulation of blood oxygenation–level dependent activation to difficulty in parametrically increasing working memory (WM) load (0-, 2-, 3-, and 4-back conditions). First, we examined parametric increases and decreases in activation to increasing WM load (positive modulation effect and negative modulation effect). Second, we examined the effect of age on modulation to difficulty (WM load) to identify regions that differed with age as difficulty increased (age-related positive and negative modulation effects). Weakened modulation to difficulty with age was found in both the positive modulation (middle frontal, superior/inferior parietal) and negative modulation effect (deactivated) regions (insula, cingulate, medial superior frontal, fusiform, and parahippocampal gyri, hippocampus, and lateral occipital cortex). Age-related alterations to positive modulation emerged later in the lifespan than negative modulation. Furthermore, these effects were significantly coupled in that greater upmodulation was associated with lesser downmodulation. Importantly, greater fronto-parietal upmodulation to difficulty and greater downmodulation of deactivated regions were associated with better task accuracy and upmodulation with better WM span measured outside the scanner. These findings suggest that greater dynamic range of modulation of activation to cognitive challenge is in service of current task performance, as well as generalizing to cognitive ability beyond the scanner task, lending support to its utility as a marker of successful cognitive aging.

Introduction

As we age, most fluid cognitive functions decline, including one of the most fundamental cognitive skills, working memory (WM) (Babcock and Salthouse, 1990, Park et al., 2002, Salthouse, 1994, Van der Linden et al., 1994). Both manipulation of items in WM (Dobbs and Rule, 1989) and the updating of this information (Artuso et al., 2016, Clarys et al., 2009, Hartman et al., 2001, Van der Linden et al., 1994) decline with increasing age. The n-back paradigm has been widely utilized in behavioral and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of WM (Owen et al., 2005, Rottschy et al., 2012), as it requires participants to monitor and flexibly update items kept in WM, and because WM load can be parametrically increased to examine change in blood oxygenation–level dependent (BOLD) response to increasing cognitive demand. WM robustly engages regions of bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior parietal cortex (PPC), cingulate gyrus, and lateral cerebellar cortex (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000). The n-back task consistently activates regions of the cognitive control network: premotor, middle frontal gyrus, anterior cingulate gyrus, and PPC (Owen et al., 2005). In younger adults, increasing WM load is associated with increasing modulation of activation in these fronto-parietal regions (Manoach et al., 1997).

Parametric n-back tasks have been utilized across various populations and contexts (Braver et al., 1997, Callicott et al., 1999, Choo et al., 2005, Cohen et al., 1997, Druzgal and D'Esposito, 2001, Jansma et al., 2004, Jonides et al., 1997) including the study of normal aging (Cappell et al., 2010, Heinzel et al., 2014, Heinzel et al., 2016, Mattay et al., 2006, Nagel et al., 2009, Nyberg et al., 2009, Rypma and D'Esposito, 2000, Sala-Llonch et al., 2012, Schulze et al., 2011). In general (with the exception of Kaup et al. (2014) and Wishart et al. (2006)), aging studies rely on comparison of extreme age groups (i.e., young vs. old) to examine age differences in modulation to increasing WM load, rather than examining the entire adult lifespan. These comparisons find unilateral prefrontal cortex response in younger adults, but bilateral frontal activation in older adults, with some studies finding greater activation in old compared to younger adults, at lower (e.g., 1-back and 2-back) WM loads (Mattay et al., 2006, Prakash et al., 2012, Reuter-Lorenz et al., 2000, Schneider-Garces et al., 2010). These extreme age group comparisons, however, omit 2 important portions of the lifespan, namely middle-age and very old adulthood. In a growing number of recent studies (Ankudowich et al., 2016, Chan et al., 2014, Grady et al., 2006, Kennedy et al., 2015, Kwon et al., 2016, Park et al., 2013, Rieck et al., 2017), middle age has been illustrated to be an essential piece of information in determining when functional brain changes occur in adulthood. Structurally, the fronto-parietal regions included in the canonical WM network are known to degrade with age around this time in both gray and white matter (Kennedy and Raz, 2009, Raz et al., 2005), and these declines are related to poorer cognition (Kennedy and Raz, 2009, Raz and Rodrigue, 2006).

Recent research has pinpointed middle age as a time when important functional changes in modulation to difficulty appear (Kennedy et al., 2015, Rieck et al., 2017). Using a spatial distance judgment paradigm, Rieck et al. (2017) found reduction in dynamic range of BOLD modulation to parametrically increasing difficulty in both fronto-parietal upmodulation (i.e., the ability to increase activation from easier to more difficult conditions) and in downmodulation (i.e., ability to increase deactivation from easier to more difficult conditions) of deactivated regions (such as ventromedial prefrontal cortex) with age. This reduced dynamic range was evident in early middle age for downmodulated regions, but not evident until older age in upmodulated brain regions, suggesting that these modulatory processes follow different aging trajectories. Interestingly, it is increasingly hypothesized that these processes are not independent, but act in synergy. Turner and Spreng (2015) have hypothesized that aging brings about an increased coupling of activation of these “default” (i.e., medial frontal, anterior and posterior cingulate, and hippocampal formation) and “executive” (i.e., dorsolateral prefrontal, posterior parietal, and cingulate) systems, or Default-Executive Coupling Hypothesis of Aging, possibly as an adaptive support for declining fluid abilities with aging. Rieck et al. (2017) found significant coupling of upmodulated and downmodulated regions, in that individuals who showed greater modulation in one direction also showed greater modulation in the other direction. Interestingly, greater coupling was associated with higher fluid intelligence. To be able to interpret the nature of alterations in BOLD data, that is, whether they are beneficial, detrimental, or unrelated to performance, these differences need to be yoked to cognition, ideally to both task performance during scanning and to measures of related cognitive processes assessed outside of the scanner (Grady, 2012).

Here, we aimed to address these issues in the existing literature by examining the full adult lifespan, with substantial sample size, utilizing a richer parametric increase in WM load than generally used to determine the nature of age-related alterations in dynamic range by yoking BOLD modulation to task performance and out-of-scanner cognition. Thus, the current study sought to characterize age-related differences in modulation to parametrically increasing WM load in a large lifespan sample of healthy adults. Specifically, we aimed to examine when in the lifespan alterations to upmodulation and downmodulation occur, whether and how these shifts in dynamic range are coupled, and whether they are related to task performance and generalize to WM beyond the scanner environment. To test this, 171 individuals aged 20–94 years underwent fMRI scanning during a digit n-back paradigm with blocks of incrementally increasing WM load: 0-, 2-, 3-, and 4-back, allowing us to model both age and WM load as continuous variables. We hypothesized that both positive and negative modulation to difficulty would decrease with age, that this modulation would be significantly coupled, and that greater dynamic range of modulation would be associated with better WM, both during scanning and on a test of WM span.

Section snippets

Participants

Participants consisted of 171 individuals aged 20–94 years (mean age = 53.03 ± 19.13 years; 100 women; 71 men) recruited from the greater Dallas metro area via media advertisements and flyers. All participants received compensation for their time. Prior to enrollment, participants completed a health history screening, telephone, and in-person interviews. All individuals were screened against neurological, psychiatric, metabolic, and cardiovascular disease (except for controlled essential

Task performance

Accuracy and response time (RT) were recorded for all trials. To examine performance differences across WM load levels and potential age effects, 2 repeated-measures GLMs were tested with WM load serving as a 4-level within-subject repeated measure and age (mean centered) as a continuous between-subjects measure predicting either mean accuracy or median RT. For accuracy, we found significant effects of age (F (1,169) = 33.89, p < 0.001) and WM load (F (3,507) =373.65, p < 0.001), with accuracy

Discussion

The present study examined how the aging brain responds to incrementally increasing WM load during a parametric n-back task in a large, lifespan sample of adults. Across our sample, engagement in a difficult WM task elicited activation in bilateral fronto-parietal regions, dorsal cingulate gyrus, precuneus, thalamus, midbrain, caudate and cerebellum (positive modulation effect), as well as deactivation (negative modulation effect) in regions such as bilateral posterior cingulate gyrus, medial

Conclusions

In sum, by utilizing the entire parametric range of conditions in an n-back design and by sampling a wide age range of individuals in a large sample, we were able to demonstrate reliable associations among aging, BOLD modulation to difficulty, and the relationships with not only task accuracy but also with out-of-scanner cognition. These findings, taken together suggest that although both positive and negative modulations decrease across the adult lifespan, maintenance of this increased dynamic

Disclosures statement

The authors have no actual or potential conflicts of interest.

Acknowledgements

This study was supported in part by grants from the National Institutes of Health, R00 AG-03618 and R00 AG-03648. The authors would like to thank Andy Hebrank for assistance with fMRI task programming, Asha Unni for behavioral task piloting and fMRI data collection, and Marci Horn for cognitive data collection.

References (72)

  • J.M. Jansma et al.

    Working memory capacity in schizophrenia: a parametric fMRI study

    Schizophr. Res.

    (2004)
  • K.M. Kennedy et al.

    Aging white matter and cognition: differential effects of regional variations in diffusion properties on memory, executive functions, and speed

    Neuropsychologia

    (2009)
  • K.M. Kennedy et al.

    Age trajectories of functional activation under conditions of low and high processing demands: an adult lifespan fMRI study of the aging brain

    Neuroimage

    (2015)
  • H.-C. Leung et al.

    Load response functions in the human spatial working memory circuit during location memory updating

    Neuroimage

    (2007)
  • V.S. Mattay et al.

    Neurophysiological correlates of age-related changes in working memory capacity

    Neurosci. Lett.

    (2006)
  • K.K. Ng et al.

    Reduced functional segregation between the default mode network and the executive control network in healthy older adults: a longitudinal study

    Neuroimage

    (2016)
  • H. Park et al.

    An fMRI study of episodic encoding across the lifespan: changes in subsequent memory effects are evident by middle-age

    Neuropsychologia

    (2013)
  • J.W. Peirce

    PsychoPy–psychophysics software in Python

    J. Neurosci. Methods

    (2007)
  • R.S. Prakash et al.

    Age-related differences in cortical recruitment and suppression: implications for cognitive performance

    Behav. Brain Res.

    (2012)
  • N. Raz et al.

    Differential aging of the brain: patterns, cognitive correlates and modifiers

    Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.

    (2006)
  • J.R. Rieck et al.

    Age-related reduction of BOLD modulation to cognitive difficulty predicts poorer task accuracy and poorer fluid reasoning ability

    Neuroimage

    (2017)
  • C. Rottschy et al.

    Modelling neural correlates of working memory: a coordinate-based meta-analysis

    Neuroimage

    (2012)
  • R. Sala-Llonch et al.

    Brain connectivity during resting state and subsequent working memory task predicts behavioural performance

    Cortex

    (2012)
  • F. Sambataro et al.

    Age-related alterations in default mode network: impact on working memory performance

    Neurobiol. Aging

    (2010)
  • R.N. Spreng et al.

    Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition

    Neuroimage

    (2010)
  • C. Artuso et al.

    Updating working memory: memory load matters with aging

    Aging Clin. Exp. Res.

    (2016)
  • R.L. Babcock et al.

    Effects of increased processing demands on age differences in working memory

    Psychol. Aging

    (1990)
  • M. Brett et al.

    Region of interest analysis using the MarsBar toolbox for SPM 99

    Poster session presented at the Functional Mapping of the Human Brain

    (2002)
  • R.L. Buckner et al.

    The brain's default network: anatomy, function, and relevance to disease

    Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci.

    (2008)
  • R. Cabeza et al.

    Imaging cognition II: an empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies

    J. Cogn. Neurosci.

    (2000)
  • J.H. Callicott et al.

    Physiological characteristics of capacity constraints in working memory as revealed by functional MRI

    Cereb. Cortex

    (1999)
  • M.Y. Chan et al.

    Decreased segregation of brain systems across the healthy adult lifespan

    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.

    (2014)
  • D. Clarys et al.

    Ageing, remembering, and executive function

    Memory

    (2009)
  • J.D. Cohen et al.

    Temporal dynamics of brain activation during a working memory task

    Nature

    (1997)
  • J.S. Damoiseaux et al.

    Reduced resting-state brain activity in the “default network” in normal aging

    Cereb. Cortex

    (2008)
  • A.R. Dobbs et al.

    Adult age differences in working memory

    Psychol. Aging

    (1989)
  • Cited by (37)

    • Reduced modulation of BOLD variability as a function of cognitive load in healthy aging

      2022, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      Our findings also mirror work examining how mean BOLD response during cognitive control tasks is modulated in response to differing task demands or conditions. Specifically, older adults show a reduced range of mean BOLD response when cognitive demands increase, such that fronto-parietal cortex is under-recruited (Cappell et al., 2010; Grady et al., 2020; Kennedy et al., 2017, 2015; Schneider-Garces et al., 2010), whereas activity in default regions (Kennedy et al., 2017; Persson et al., 2007; Rieck et al., 2017; Sambataro et al., 2010) is less dampened. Together with the current study, the BOLD variability and mean functional activity findings suggest that fronto-parietal control and default regions are generally less responsive to increased cognitive demands in old age.

    • Age-related dedifferentiation and hyperdifferentiation of perceptual and mnemonic representations

      2021, Neurobiology of Aging
      Citation Excerpt :

      We also found an age-related ERS reduction in the hippocampus (Fig. 4A). This finding is consistent with evidence of impaired hippocampal activity in OAs (Kennedy et al., 2017; Nyberg, 2017), and it extends this evidence to multivariate activation patterns. Besides normal aging, the use of ERS to investigate hippocampal memory representations could be useful for investigating Alzheimer's disease, in which the hippocampus structure and function are known to be particularly compromised (Frisoni et al., 2010; Dickerson and Sperling, 2008; Wang et al., 2006; Rathore et al., 2017).

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    1

    This author is now at Rotman Research Institute, Baycrest Centre, Toronto, ON, Canada.

    View full text