Report
Reflexively mindblind: Using theory of mind to interpret behavior requires effortful attention

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2009.12.019Get rights and content

Abstract

People commonly interpret others’ behavior in terms of the actors’ underlying beliefs, knowledge, or other mental states, thereby using their “theory of mind.” Two experiments suggest that using one’s theory of mind is a relatively effortful process. In both experiments, people reflexively used their own knowledge and beliefs to follow a speaker’s instruction, but only effortfully used their theory of mind to take into account a speaker’s intention to interpret those instructions. In Experiment 1, people with lower working memory capacity were less effective than people with larger working memory capacity in applying their theory of mind to interpret behavior. In Experiment 2, an attention-demanding secondary task reduced people’s ability to apply their theory of mind. People appear to be reflexively mindblind, interpreting behavior in terms of the actor’s mental states only to the extent that they have the cognitive resources to do so.

Introduction

By age two, humans have acquired a capacity for social intelligence that outperforms our nearest primate relatives—the capacity to interpret others’ actions in terms of intentions, beliefs, knowledge, and other mental states (Herrmann, Call, Hernandez-Lloreda, Hare, & Tomasello, 2007). This capacity—generally referred to as “theory of mind”—continues to develop through adulthood, at which point it seems almost inevitable that people will interpret others’ actions in terms of underlying mental states. A man running down the street is not simply seen as an object moving in space but rather as a motivated and thoughtful agent. His behavior is understood differently if we know that he is wanting to get some exercise than if we know that he is wanting to elude the police. Understanding a person’s actions often requires considering an agent’s beliefs, and in this sense people appear to use their theory of mind spontaneously to interpret others’ actions.

There are good reasons to believe that the spontaneous use of beliefs to interpret action is effortless. Consider, for example, how people interpret language. The back and forth of conversation occurs quickly, requiring people to understand and formulate utterances without much time to reflect. To understand what others are saying, people constantly resolve the ambiguity inherent in the language so that they can use what speakers believe in order to understand what they mean. The ease of conversation has led to the conclusion that people’s “Use of theory of mind is also rapid … automatic, requiring no effortful attention … and universal.” (Stone, Baron-Cohen, & Knight, 1998, p. 640).

However, there are also good reasons to believe that the use of beliefs to interpret action is effortful. A person’s own perspective is likely to be primary. Considering another’s differing perspective requires both time and motivation (e.g., Epley, Keysar, Van Boven, & Gilovich, 2004), and people appear less able to imagine themselves in another person’s perspective when they are distracted by a concurrent processing task (Davis, Conklin, Smith, & Luce, 1996). Actively monitoring another person’s belief in the midst of conversation also appears to require attentional resources (Apperly et al., 2006, Vorauer et al., 2009), and people are more likely to correctly use knowledge about others’ beliefs to predict their actions when put in a negative mood that stimulates more elaborate thinking (Converse, Lin, Keysar, & Epley, 2008). If inferring another’s differing perspective and monitoring others’ beliefs in interaction are both effortful processes, then using what is known about another’s beliefs to interpret their action may be effortful as well.

We examined in two experiments whether people effortlessly use their knowledge about another’s beliefs to interpret their actions. Based on existing research in the effort required to infer and track others’ beliefs, we predicted that people would be reflexively mindblind (Baron-Cohen, 1995), meaning that they would fail to consider others’ beliefs and knowledge to interpret their behavior unless they had sufficient attentional resources to do so.

Section snippets

Clarifying concepts

Language is often ambiguous, and it is not unusual for the same term to be used by different researchers to describe very different processes. In this research, we examine how people use what they know about another’s beliefs to interpret what he or she says. This involves employing one’s knowledge about how beliefs relate to behaviors, such as speaking, a process that entails using one’s “theory of mind” (Wellman, Cross, & Watson, 2001).

This is not to be confused or equated with the broader

Using a theory of mind: beyond active perspective taking

Having a theory of mind is crucial for social functioning because it enables people to anticipate and interpret others’ actions based on what is known about another’s beliefs, attitudes, or knowledge (Perner, 1991, Wellman et al., 2001). Because behavior, such as spoken language, is often ambiguous, it is critical to consider another’s mental states in order to interpret their actions. Saying that a math question is “easy,” for instance, is likely to mean very different things coming from a

Experiment 1: working memory

People vary in their working memory capacity (Just & Carpenter, 1992), with those high in capacity having more available resources to devote to attention-demanding processes than those lower in working memory capacity. If using theory of mind is an effortful process, then people with less working memory capacity should have more difficulty using it in the referential communication task than people with more working memory capacity.

Recall that directors, in the example just described, can see

Experiment 2: cognitive load

Experiment 1 investigated how naturally occurring variability in working memory is related to theory of mind use. Such individual differences in working memory, however, may co-vary with other factors that also influence theory of mind use, such as general intelligence. Experiment 2, therefore, sought convergent evidence by experimentally manipulating participants’ ability to expend attentional resources, rather than simply measuring them.

We manipulated the availability of resources by adding a

General discussion

The ability to reason about others’ mental states appears to be the mental capacity that makes humans uniquely intelligent (Herrmann et al., 2007). Possessing a capacity and actually using it to interpret social action, however, are two different things. Two experiments demonstrated that people’s ability to use their theory of mind depended on their capacity to expend effortful attentional resources, in particular on their working memory. In Experiment 1, people with low working memory capacity

Acknowledgments

Funding was provided by a National Institute of Health Grant R01 MH49685-06A1 and the Templeton Foundation. We thank Travis Carter, Erica Kees, Robin Lassonde, Regina Lewandoski, Sixto Munoz, Dawn Perry and Kenny Yu for technical help.

References (34)

  • B.A. Converse et al.

    In the mood to get over yourself: Mood affects theory-of-mind use

    Emotion

    (2008)
  • M.H. Davis et al.

    Effect of perspective taking on the cognitive representation of persons: A merging of self and other

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1996)
  • D.C. Dennett

    Beliefs about beliefs

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (1978)
  • I. Dumontheil et al.

    Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence

    Developmental Science

    (2009)
  • N. Epley et al.

    When perspective taking increases taking: Reactive egoism in social interaction

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2006)
  • N. Epley et al.

    Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • D.T. Gilbert et al.

    The trouble of thinking: Activation and application of stereotypic beliefs

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1991)
  • Cited by (260)

    • Visuospatial perspective taking in people with Down syndrome

      2023, Research in Developmental Disabilities
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text