Infants use attention but not emotions to predict others’ actions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.11.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Phillips et al. (2002) suggest that by 12–14 months, infants can use a person's emotional and attentional cues to predict that person's actions. However, this work was conducted using only positive emotions, which is problematic because attention and positive emotions lead to the same prediction about a person's actions, thus leaving unclear whether infants made predictions based upon attention and emotion or attention alone. To get around this problem, we used both positive and negative emotions in a looking-time paradigm to investigate whether 14-month-old infants can use emotional cues to predict a person's actions. The findings suggest that infants used attentional but not emotional cues as predictors. We argue that while 14-month-olds can use another person's emotion cues to modify their own behavior (as in social referencing situations), they do not yet use them robustly to predict the other's behavior.

Section snippets

Participants

A total of 59 14-month-old infants were tested. The infants’ parents had been contacted by advertisements or mailings, and were offered $10 as travel reimbursement. Eleven of the 59 infants were excluded because they were fussy or distracted (7), there was an experimenter error (2) or an equipment failure (1), or because the infant was an outlier1

Familiarization

To assess infants’ looking to the happy versus disgust emotions during familiarization events, we conducted a repeated-measures ANOVA with duration of looks as the dependent measure, trial as a within-subjects factor (with 4 levels: familiarization trials 1, 2, 3, and 4), and condition as a between-subjects factor (with 2 levels: happy and disgust). This analysis revealed a main effect of condition, F(1,46) = 9.839, p = .003: infants’ looking times during disgust familiarization events (M = 23.39 s, SD

Discussion

Infants saw an actress looking either happily or disgustedly into a cup while ignoring a different cup. If infants can use emotions to reason and make predictions about others’ actions, they should show greater novelty preference when the actress behaves in a manner inconsistent with her emotional displays. That is, they should look longer not only when the actress has looked happily into one cup and then reaches into the other cup but also when the actress has looked disgustedly into one cup

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the parents and children who participated in the study. We also thank Özlem Ece Demir, Tobias Grossmann, and Kalina Michalska for helpful discussions, and the members of the Center for Infant Studies at the University of Chicago for their help in conducting the studies. This research was supported by NIH grant HD35707 to the second author.

References (23)

  • S.J. Hespos et al.

    Reasoning about containment events in very young infants

    Cognition

    (2001)
  • A.T. Phillips et al.

    Infants’ ability to connect gaze and emotional expression to intentional action

    Cognition

    (2002)
  • H.M. Wellman et al.

    From simple desires to ordinary beliefs: The early development of everyday psychology

    Cognition

    (1990)
  • J.T. Cacioppo et al.

    Beyond bipolar conceptualizations and measures: The case of attitudes and evaluative space

    Personality and Social Psychology Review

    (1997)
  • A.J. Caron et al.

    Infant understanding of containment: An affordance perceived or a relationship conceived?

    Developmental Psychology

    (1988)
  • P. Ekman et al.

    Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues

    (1975)
  • G. Gergely et al.

    On pedagogy

    Developmental Science

    (2007)
  • M.J. Hertenstein et al.

    The retention effects of an adult's emotional displays on infant behavior

    Child Development

    (2004)
  • R. Hornik et al.

    The effects of maternal positive, neutral, and negative affective communication on infant responses to new toys

    Child Development

    (1987)
  • L.J. Moses et al.

    Evidence for referential understanding in the emotions domain at twelve and eighteen months

    Child Development

    (2001)
  • D.L. Mumme et al.

    Infants’ responses to facial and vocal emotional signals in a social referencing paradigm

    Child Development

    (1996)
  • Cited by (42)

    • Addressing joint action challenges in HRI: Insights from psychology and philosophy

      2022, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      Another way of establishing mutual recognition through non-verbal communication is to rely on ostensive acts, which can be regarded as non-verbal observatives. Notice for example how humans, from early infancy, are capable of predicting a course of action based on a target's hand movement (Koch & Stapel, 2019), sustained visual attention (Vaish & Woodward, 2010), or contextual constraints (Gergely et al., 2002). These capabilities are often used to build communicative strategies.

    • Empathic accuracy of young boys and girls in ongoing parent–child interactions: Performance and (mis)perception

      2021, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
      Citation Excerpt :

      Given infants’ limited cognitive and physical abilities, nonverbal tasks requiring an inference of social understanding based on primitive responses such as whether the infants like or dislike a stimulus or find a scenario surprising are generally used in such research. However, it is possible that the infants are responding to behavioral cues rather than representing others’ minds (Vaish & Woodward, 2010), making interpretation a challenge. Theory of mind (ToM) refers to the ability to infer another’s mental state and use it to explain and predict behavior and is sometimes equated with cognitive empathy (McDonald & Messinger, 2011).

    • Social Cognition

      2020, Encyclopedia of Infant and Early Childhood Development
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text