Elsevier

Cognition

Volume 133, Issue 2, November 2014, Pages 474-479
Cognition

Neighborhood linguistic diversity predicts infants’ social learning

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2014.08.002Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Neighborhood linguistic diversity predicts infants’ imitation of foreign-language speakers.

  • The infants included in the study heard only English from parents and caretakers.

  • Family income and neighborhood population density did not affect infants’ imitation.

  • The findings reveal unique effects of community diversity on infant learning.

Abstract

Infants’ direct interactions with caregivers have been shown to powerfully influence social and cognitive development. In contrast, little is known about the cognitive influence of social contexts beyond the infant’s immediate interactions with others, for example, the communities in which infants live. The current study addressed this issue by asking whether neighborhood linguistic diversity predicts infants’ propensity to learn from diverse social partners. Data were taken from a series of experiments in which 19-month-old infants from monolingual, English-speaking homes were tested in paradigms that assessed their tendency to imitate the actions of an adult who spoke either English or Spanish. Infants who lived in more linguistically diverse neighborhoods imitated more of the Spanish speaker’s actions. This relation was observed in two separate datasets and found to be independent from variation in infants’ general imitative abilities, age, median family income and population density. These results provide novel evidence suggesting that infants’ social learning is predicted by the diversity of the communities in which they live.

Introduction

Social environments powerfully shape early cognitive development. A large body of research has demonstrated that infants’ immediate social interactions with parents, teachers or caregivers influence diverse cognitive achievements, including language learning (e.g., Hoff, 2003, Huttenlocher et al., 2002, Rowe, 2012), spatial cognition (e.g., Pruden, Levine, & Huttenlocher, 2011), theory of mind (e.g., Meins et al., 2002), number knowledge (e.g., Levine, Suriyakham, Rowe, Huttenlocher, & Gunderson, 2010), and culturally-specified practices (see Rogoff, Paradise, Mejía Arauz, Correa-Chávez, & Angelillo, 2003). In contrast, little is known about the cognitive influence of social contexts beyond the infant’s immediate interactions with others. Dominant perspectives on early social cognitive development have stressed the central importance of infants’ direct interactions with social partners (e.g., Carpendale and Lewis, 2004, Csibra and Gergely, 2009, Dunn, 1988, Tomasello, 1998), and consequently there has been little investigation of the influence that distal social contexts may have. Nevertheless, infants routinely experience their broader neighborhood environment, for example, at the park, on the bus, or in the supermarket. Do these experiences affect their social cognitive development? In the current study, we investigated this issue by asking whether neighborhood linguistic diversity affects infants’ propensity to learn from diverse social partners.

One way in which neighborhood demographics could influence young learners is by shaping their openness to social informants. Recent findings indicate that infants and young children are discriminating social learners-they resist attending to and taking information from foreign or foreign-accented speakers (Buttelmann et al., 2013, Howard et al., in press, Howard et al., in preparation, Kinzler et al., 2011, Kinzler et al., 2007). Thus, from early in life, infants and young children appear to form expectations about the kinds of people that they should learn from and imitate. While this tendency could reflect a drive to acquire socially relevant knowledge (Henderson, Sabbagh, & Woodward, 2013), it could also restrict children’s access to potentially valuable information and contribute to the development of social biases.

We recruited data from 4 prior experiments with 19-month-old infants in order to evaluate whether neighborhood diversity mitigates this learning bias in infants. The 4 experiments were drawn from two sets of studies that examined age and medium effects on infants’ willingness to imitate informants who spoke their own native language (English) versus a foreign language (Spanish) (Howard et al., in press, Howard et al., in preparation). These studies found that infants and young children resisted foreign-speaking informants in some cases, but also found that, when presented with a live (rather than video) informant, 19-month-old infants were equally likely to imitate the actions of Spanish- and English-speaking experimenters. In the analyses presented here, we pooled data from these experiments to evaluate whether variation in neighborhood linguistic diversity predicted infants’ responses to the foreign speaker. We selected infants who heard only English in their interactions with caretakers. These infants lived in neighborhoods with varying degrees of linguistic diversity. By examining the relation between neighborhood linguistic diversity and infants’ propensity to imitate the foreign speaker, we were able to test whether language information available outside of the home affects infants’ social learning. That is, these experiments provided the opportunity to isolate the potential effects of the social environment beyond the child’s immediate interactions with caretakers and family members.

Section snippets

Participants

Data were drawn from four experiments investigating 19-month-old infants’ imitation of native-versus foreign-language speakers (Howard et al., in press, Howard et al., in preparation). Participants were full-term 19-month-old infants from English-speaking monolingual households in the Washington, D.C. and Chicago metro areas. Participant ages and demographic information are summarized in Table 1.

All participants heard a minimum of 95% English in their daily lives, and heard only English from

Results

Preliminary analyses confirmed that infants in these samples did not differ, overall, in their imitation of the Spanish- and English-speaking presenters (see Table 3), and that infants in the two between-subjects conditions (dataset 1) did not significantly differ in their demographic characteristics (see Table 1). Further, although infants were selected to have very little, if any, exposure to a language other than English, we confirmed that this exposure was not reliably correlated with

Discussion

The current findings provide novel evidence demonstrating that infants’ social learning is shaped by the diversity of the neighborhoods in which they live, independent of direct interactions with caretakers and family members. That is, infants’ incidental exposure to linguistic diversity in neighborhood situations (such as parks, bus rides, or visits to the grocery store) influenced their propensity to learn from outgroup members. Infants who lived in more linguistically diverse neighborhoods

Author note

This procedures outlined in this manuscript were approved by the first author’s institutional review board and adhere to the principles found in the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki.

This research was supported, in part, by NICHD grant R01-HD035707 to the third author.

References (35)

  • G. Csibra et al.

    Natural pedagogy

    Trends in Cognitive Sciences

    (2009)
  • A.M.E. Henderson et al.

    Preschoolers’ selective learning is guided by the principle of relevance

    Cognition

    (2013)
  • J. Huttenlocher et al.

    Language input and child syntax

    Cognitive Psychology

    (2002)
  • Y. Bar-Haim et al.

    Nature and nurture in own-race face processing

    Psychological Science

    (2006)
  • D. Buttelmann et al.

    Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants

    Child Development

    (2013)
  • J. Carpendale et al.

    Constructing an understanding of mind: The development of children’s social understanding within social interaction

    Behavioral and Brain Sciences

    (2004)
  • P. Chavajay et al.

    Cultural variation in management of attention by children and their caregivers

    Developmental Psychology

    (1999)
  • J. Dunn

    The beginnings of social understanding

    (1988)
  • A. Fernald et al.

    Common themes and cultural variations in Japanese and American mothers’ speech to infants

    Child Development

    (1993)
  • P. Floor et al.

    Can 18-month-old infants learn words by listening in on conversations?

    Infancy

    (2006)
  • A. Gampe et al.

    Eighteen-month-olds learn novel words through overhearding

    First Language

    (2012)
  • S. Gaskins

    Children’s daily lives in a Mayan village: A case study of culturally constructed roles and activities

  • S. Gaskins et al.

    Learning through observation

  • E. Hoff

    The specificity of environmental influence: Socioeconomic status affects early vocabulary development via maternal speech

    Child Development

    (2003)
  • Howard, Henderson, Carrazza & Woodward (in press). Infants’ and young children’s imitation of linguistic ingroup and...
  • Howard, Henderson, & Woodward (2014). Do infants prefer linguistic ingroup members over linguistic outgroup members?...
  • Killen, M., Rutland, A., & Ruck, M. (2011). Promoting equity, tolerance, and justice: Policy implications. In SRCD...
  • Cited by (42)

    • Children's evaluations of culturally diverse lunchbox foods

      2024, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology
    • How do natural environments shape adaptive cognition across the lifespan?

      2022, Trends in Cognitive Sciences
      Citation Excerpt :

      Such measurements could take the form of dynamic, continuously recorded data (e.g., geolocation data and records of social contacts), or compiled statistics that summarize important features of the environment (e.g., location-specific sociodemographic data). For example, geolocation data have been used to test the hypothesis that day-to-day variation in novelty exposure modulates affective states [8], and information about infants’ sociodemographic environments has been used to examine the relation between the diversity of linguistic input and learning from diverse social interaction partners [9]. While these studies are a good starting point, they could be expanded to track behavior across extended timescales to reveal processes of dynamic change over the lifespan.

    • Pragmatic and knowledge range lenience towards foreigners

      2022, Acta Psychologica
      Citation Excerpt :

      Foreign-accented speech may not only affect the message's intelligibility, but it may also lead to an implicit categorization of the speaker as an outgroup individual (foreign) in terms of cultural and social heritage. Recent evidence shows that such categorization based on speech is an implicit and automatic process (Baus et al., 2017; Baus et al., 2021; Champoux-Larsson et al., 2021; Kinzler et al., 2010; Pietraszewski & Schwartz, 2014) and emerges in the first years of life (Begus et al., 2016; Howard et al., 2014; Liberman et al., 2017). Importantly, the classification of a person as an outgroup member entails the activation of the socio-cultural stereotypes associated with foreign groups and this in turn affects the trustworthiness of the message (Giles, 2013; Ryan, 1983; Stevenage et al., 2012; for evidence with children, see Kinzler & DeJesus, 2013).

    • Learning and development in infancy: toward an integrated approach

      2022, International Encyclopedia of Education: Fourth Edition
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text