Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1597, 9 February 2015, Pages 119-128
Brain Research

Research Report
Visual influences on sensorimotor EEG responses during observation of hand actions

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.11.048Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Hand used to perform the action influenced alpha suppression for egocentric actions.

  • Perspective of viewed actions influenced alpha suppression for right-handed actions.

  • An anticipatory effect of perspective occurred prior the onset of the hand actions.

Abstract

There is growing interest within the field of social-cognitive neuroscience in the dynamics of sensorimotor EEG rhythms during the observation of actions performed by others. However, there remain important gaps in the literature regarding the effects of perceptual aspects of the observed hand movements. This study investigated two visual influences on the EEG response to hand actions. Specifically, the perspective of the action in relation to the participant (egocentric/allocentric) was varied and the effect of the hand used to carry out the action (left/right) was also assessed. While EEG was recorded, 28 undergraduate participants observed video clips showing an actor’s hand reaching for, grasping, and lifting a cylindrical object across four conditions (right-hand egocentric, left-hand egocentric, right-hand allocentric, and left-hand allocentric). For actions viewed from an egocentric perspective, significantly greater event-related desynchronization (ERD) was present in the 7–9 Hz range over right mid-frontal, right central, and bilateral mid-parietal sites for right-handed actions compared to left-handed actions. In addition, greater ERD was observed within the 7–9 Hz band during the observation of right-handed egocentric actions compared to actions viewed from the allocentric perspective. This finding was present at bilateral central and mid-parietal sites, and emerged as an anticipatory effect prior to the onset of the observed hand movements.

Introduction

There continues to be a good deal of interest across the field of social-cognitive neuroscience in delineating the brain networks that are active during the observation of others’ actions (Molenberghs et al., 2012). This interest has been partly driven by the suggestion that neural processes involved in executing an action are also active while watching others perform a similar action (Rizzolatti and Sinigaglia, 2010). Although the nature and function of these neural overlaps remain a topic of debate, they have been proposed as being important for understanding the processes involved in human social learning and communication (Keysers and Perrett, 2004, Kourtis et al., 2013). Learning new skills, perceiving the goals and intentions of others, and communicating via gestures all involve an understanding that can be facilitated by observing the actions of others. By gaining basic knowledge about the neural processing involved in action observation, we can also work toward understanding how these processes may come to be impaired in certain disorders.

Patterns of brain activation during action observation have been studied using various imaging methodologies, including the electroencephalogram (EEG). Related studies of action processing have placed a particular focus on sensorimotor brain rhythms such as the mu rhythm, which oscillates in the alpha frequency range over central electrode sites (Pfurtscheller and Lopes da Silva, 1999, Pineda, 2005). The mu rhythm has been used to examine the coupling between action perception and action production through examining similarities in patterns of mu desynchronization between the observation of an action and the execution of a similar action (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2004, Orgs et al., 2008, Perry et al., 2011). However, despite sustained interest in the responses of mu and other sensorimotor rhythms (e.g., beta) during the observation of others’ actions, there remain various basic questions about the patterning of EEG activity during action observation. One such question concerns how the response of the mu rhythm to action observation may be modulated by the visual perspective from which an action is viewed as well as which hand (left vs. right) the person carrying out the action is using. In the current study, the response of the mu rhythm to observed hand actions was assessed to probe specific questions about how these responses vary according the visual perspective from which the action was observed, as well as the hand (left vs. right) that was used to carry out the action.

One open question concerning brain activity during action observation is how the perspective, or viewpoint, of an observation affects the neural processing of the action. Evidence from recordings of mirror neurons in premotor areas of macaque monkeys suggests that the activity of approximately three quarters of the neurons in these areas during action observation is dependent on the perspective from which a grasping action was viewed by the monkey (Caggiano et al., 2011). In humans, EEG studies have examined differences in responses between viewing actions from an egocentric (first-person) perspective vs. an allocentric (third-person) perspective, although results have been inconsistent. In one study, greater mu rhythm suppression was apparent when participants viewed reaching and grasping actions from an allocentric perspective compared to when they viewed similar actions from an egocentric perspective (Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2013). These authors proposed that compared with actions viewed from an egocentric perspective, the visuospatial transformation required for actions viewed from an allocentric perspective involves a heavier processing load, which may account for greater mu suppression during allocentric actions. They also suggested that an allocentric perspective would present a more familiar perspective than an egocentric perspective, which could also account for greater mu suppression during the observation of allocentric actions.

In a similar EEG study, Fu and Franz (2014) had participants view simple action scenes in which both hands were visible and one hand moved from one spot to another in a reaching trajectory (but without the presence of an object). In contrast to Frenkel-Toledo et al. (2013), the authors observed greater mu suppression over sensorimotor areas while participants viewed these hand movements from an egocentric compared to an allocentric perspective. These findings are supported by a related fMRI study by Jackson et al. (2006) who found greater activation in sensorimotor areas during the observation of hand actions presented from an egocentric perspective. These authors proposed that the first-person perspective may be more directly mapped onto the sensorimotor system, whereas information gained from an allocentric perspective would have to undergo a visuospatial transformation to be aligned with the observer’s own viewpoint.

In considering the similarities and differences in findings between these prior studies, it is worth noting the specific stimuli that were used in each invesigation. In the studies by Fu and Franz (2014) and Jackson et al. (2006), the egocentric views only showed the hand and forearm, such that participants saw a first-person view that was similar to watching themselves perform the hand movement. In addition, the actions were limited to simple hand movements, without the grasping or manipulation of an object. However, in the study by Frenkel-Toledo et al. (2013), the video clips in the egocentric condition included part of the actor’s shoulder, which changes the view from approximating that of one’s own actions to a view of someone else, as if the participant was looking over the shoulder of another person. This study also used object-directed hand actions rather than the more intransitive movements in the other reports. In the current study, we attempted to reconcile the various differences in the literature by recording EEG while participants viewed object-directed hand actions from egocentric and allocentric perspectives, where the egocentric perspective was a first-person view similar to that used in Fu and Franz (2014) and Jackson et al. (2006).

In addition to the effect of perspective on neural activity during the observation of hand actions, another unresolved question concerns the patterning of brain responses in relation to the specific hand being used to carry out an observed action. Using fMRI, early work in this area in adults suggested a somatotopic organization of sensorimotor cortex activity during action observation (Buccino et al., 2001), with recent EEG findings in infants supporting this organization and adding a developmental aspect (Saby et al., 2013). However, this work focused on contrasting activity during actions carried out with different effectors (e.g., hand vs. foot) rather than actions carried out with left vs. right hands. As with the effect of viewing perspective, studies that have addressed the question of whether mu suppression in the adult EEG varies according to the hand used to carry out the action have also been somewhat inconsistent. Some reports suggest that EEG mu suppression over sensorimotor regions is not dependent on the hand used to carry out an observed action (Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2013, Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2004), although other studies have reported greater activation in regions contralateral to the hand being observed (Perry and Bentin, 2009). If observed actions were directly mapped onto one’s sensorimotor system, a somatotopic mapping of right-handed actions to the left hemisphere and left-handed actions to the right hemisphere would be expected.

The present study investigated how patterns of EEG mu rhythm desynchronization vary while young adults observed actions that were viewed from either an allocentric or egocentric perspective and were carried out with either the left or right hand (see Fig. 1). Each observed action consisted of a video clip of a hand reaching for, grasping and lifting a cylindrical object that was displayed in the center of the screen. It was predicted that compared with the allocentric perspective, viewing the hand actions from an egocentric viewing perspective would be associated with greater suppression in the mu frequency range over sensorimotor regions, due to the more direct mapping of the egocentric (first-person) view to one’s own movements. In terms of the effect of the hand used, it was predicted that greater suppression of the mu rhythm over sensorimotor regions would occur over the hemisphere contralateral to the hand observed performing the action when contrasting right- and left-handed actions.

EEG analyses focused on the comparison of mean event-related spectral perturbation (ERSP) between viewing left vs. right hand actions within each of the allocentric and egocentric viewing conditions, and on the comparison of ERSP between allocentric and egocentric conditions for each hand used (left or right). Although ERSP was computed across a wider range of frequencies and electrode sites, the focus of the study was on changes in the 7–13 Hz range, which encompasses the mu rhythm at central sites as well as the posterior visual alpha rhythm over occipital sites. The primary analyses concerned activity in this band within a scalp region of interest (ROI), consisting of electrodes overlying sensorimotor regions (FC1, FC2, Cz, C3, C4, CP1, and CP2).

Section snippets

Behavioral performance

In order to maintain their attention to the stimuli, participants were asked to silently keep a count of the number of trials on which the video stimulus froze (as a still frame), and to report the number of freeze-frame trials after each block. Eleven percent of trials contained a freeze-frame, and these trials were not included in further analysis. Across the whole sample, reports of the number of freeze frames were correct for 96.4% of the blocks. Twenty-two of the 28 participants reported

Discussion

The goal of the current study was to investigate the ways in which the response patterns of the sensorimotor mu rhythm in the EEG signal are influenced by specific characteristics of observed actions. We compared EEG responses during the observation of goal-directed hand actions viewed from either an allocentric or egocentric perspective and carried out with either the left or right hand. Generally speaking, the onset of action (i.e., the onset of movement of the hand towards the object) was

Participants

Thirty-eight undergraduates received course credit in return for participation. Participants signed an informed consent form that had been approved by the university Institutional Review Board. Data from two participants were not included due to experimenter error, three participants were lost due to hardware problems, and five participants had excessive motor and/or eye movement artifacts that contaminated more than 25% of trials. Following these exclusions, the final sample included 28

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Daniel Appel, Angelique Frazier, Melissa Goodwin, Reanna Serafine, Nhi Tran, and Amanda Viands for assistance with data collection.

References (36)

Cited by (21)

  • Perspective-dependent activation of frontoparietal circuits during the observation of a static body effector

    2021, Brain Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Despite some mixed results (e.g., Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2013; Fitzgibbon et al., 2014), an advantage in visuomotor mapping for egocentric stimuli has been proved for moving effectors during AO. Indeed, EEG (Fu and Franz, 2014; Drew et al., 2015; Angelini et al., 2018), fMRI (e.g., Jackson et al., 2006), and TMS studies (e.g., Maeda et al., 2002) have shown stronger motor resonance induced by AO for egocentric relative to the allocentric viewpoint. Concerning body images with implied motion and depicting hand gestures, behavioral studies provided mixed results, showing both egocentric (e.g., Bruzzo et al., 2008) and allocentric advantage (e.g., Vogt et al., 2003) in terms of visuomotor priming effects.

  • What modulates the Mirror Neuron System during action observation?: Multiple factors involving the action, the actor, the observer, the relationship between actor and observer, and the context

    2021, Progress in Neurobiology
    Citation Excerpt :

    In general, the most common result is that, consistent with the monkey data reported by Caggiano et al. (2015) and Maranesi et al. (2017), MNS responses are greater for actions observed from an egocentric than an allocentric or lateral perspective. This has been found in multiple studies using fMRI (Jackson et al., 2006; Pilgramm et al., 2010; Oosterhof et al., 2012; Ge et al., 2018), EEG (Drew et al., 2015; Angelini et al., 2018), and TMS (Maeda et al., 2002). Two other studies—one using MEG (Kilner et al., 2006) and the other EEG (Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2013)—obtained what initially appear to be contrary results, namely greater MNS responses for actions observed from an allocentric than an egocentric perspective; however, in both studies the egocentric perspective was defined as seeing the whole agent from behind, so the relatively enhanced mirror activity that emerged for the allocentric perspective, which was defined as seeing the whole agent from the front, may have reflected the agent's greater social salience in that condition.

  • Motor resonance in monkey parietal and premotor cortex during action observation: Influence of viewing perspective and effector identity

    2021, NeuroImage
    Citation Excerpt :

    Still others did not find consistent differences for ego- versus allocentric action viewpoints (Alaert et al., 2009). EEG measurements either showed stronger modulations of sensorimotor cortex for first person (Drew et al., 2015; Angelini et al., 2018) or third person (Frenkel-Toledo et al., 2013). Some fMRI studies also employed multi-voxel pattern analysis techniques to study effects of viewpoints of observed actions on motor responses.

  • Intranasal oxytocin enhances EEG mu rhythm desynchronization during execution and observation of social action: An exploratory study

    2020, Psychoneuroendocrinology
    Citation Excerpt :

    Firstly, in contrast to EXE, in which stimuli were live presented, during OBS, stimuli were presented on a computer screen and in a third-person perspective (side view — 90°). Previous studies have provided evidence that motor resonance is stronger during live presentation of actions compared to the presentation of images or video-clips (Fox et al., 2016), as well as during the presentation of actions from a particular perspective, and in particular from the first-person perspective (Angelini et al., 2018; Berntsen et al., 2017; Caggiano et al., 2011; Drew et al., 2015), while weaker responses can be observed for other perspectives. These two factors could have thus made the observation task less effective in eliciting OT-related differences between experimental conditions and groups.

View all citing articles on Scopus
View full text