Elsevier

Brain Research

Volume 1586, 24 October 2014, Pages 152-161
Brain Research

Research Report
Erasing the face after-effect

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.052Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Face after-effects decay exponentially after a blank delay period.

  • Face after-effects are reduced even after brief presentation of an unrelated face.

  • Unrelated faces reduce the time constant of face after-effects by 85%.

Abstract

Perceptual after-effects decay over time at a rate that depends on several factors, such as the duration of adaptation and the duration of the test stimuli. Whether this decay is accelerated by exposure to other faces after adaptation is not known. Our goal was to determine if the appearance of other faces during a delay period after adaptation affected the face identity after-effect. In the first experiment we investigated whether, in the perception of ambiguous stimuli created by morphing between two faces, the repulsive after-effects from adaptation to one face were reduced by brief presentation of the second face in a delay period. We found no effect; however, this may have been confounded by a small attractive after-effect from the interference face. In the second experiment, the interference stimuli were faces unrelated to those used as adaptation stimuli, and we examined after-effects at three different delay periods. This showed a decline in after-effects as the time since adaptation increased, and an enhancement of this decline by the presentation of intervening faces. An exponential model estimated that the intervening faces caused an 85% reduction in the time constant of the after-effect decay. In conclusion, we confirm that face after-effects decline rapidly after adaptation and that exposure to other faces hastens the re-setting of the system.

Introduction

Exposure to a stimulus can alter perception of a following one. For example, seeing a line with a clockwise tilt causes a subsequent vertical line to appear tilted counter-clockwise (Gibson and Radner, 1937, Harris and Calvert, 1989). Such after-effects have been observed after adaptation for many types of visual properties, such as curvature (Bales and Follansbee, 1935, Gheorghiu and Kingdom, 2007, Gibson, 1933), motion (Hershenson, 1989) and contour shape (Prins, 2009).

After-effects also occur for more complex stimuli such as faces (Webster and MacLeod, 2011). For example, adaptation to a distorted face results in a subsequently viewed ‘normal’ face being perceived as distorted in the opposite direction (MacLin and Webster, 2001). Face after-effects have been shown not just for shape but also for a number of complex facial properties, such as identity (Fox et al., 2008, Leopold et al., 2001), expression (Fox and Barton, 2007), gender (Oruc et al., 2011), and age (Lai et al., 2012, Lai et al., 2013). Although face after-effects are reduced by off-setting the location of the test stimuli by 5° relative to the adaptor stimuli (Afraz and Cavanagh, 2008), substantial after-effects remain even if the retinotopic contribution of low-level image properties are reduced by varying the size (Rhodes et al., 2007) or retinal location (Leopold et al., 2001, Roach and Webb, 2013) of the adaptor in relation to the test stimulus. This is consistent with other findings that indicate that face after-effects are not derived from low-level image properties, but from high-level representations (Butler et al., 2008).

After-effects are evidence of a short-term experience-dependent form of plasticity in the visual system. The benefits of these effects to perception are not entirely clear. In motion perception it has been suggested that after-effects may be evidence of a re-calibration or gain control, to make operation under varying conditions of motion more effective (Verstraten et al., 1994). For face after-effects, there is evidence that facilitation from brief exposure is accompanied by a lateral inhibition of perception of other faces (Oruç and Barton, 2010), and that this inhibition has a center-surround organisation (Rostamirad et al., 2009), which could enhance perception of the adapted face over similar faces. With longer adaptation, there is sharpened tuning of shape discrimination specific for the adapted face (Oruc and Barton, 2011), which could again benefit the distinction of the adapted face from other competing faces. Thus, face after-effects could also be manifestations of a type of gain control to enhance processing of the face being viewed.

To be effective in daily experience such benefits should be transient, as recently viewed faces continue to be replaced by new faces in new encounters. For visual adaptation in general, there has long been evidence that after-effects decay over time. This has been shown for displacement after-effects (Hammer, 1949), for example, and has been particularly well studied for the motion after-effect, whose decay has been modelled with an exponential function (Hershenson, 1989, Keck and Pentz, 1977, Taylor, 1963). Recent studies also show a rapid decline in face identity after-effects in the first few seconds after adaptation (Leopold et al., 2001, Leopold et al., 2005, Rhodes et al., 2007). On the other hand, there are also reports that face distortion after-effects may persist at a modest level for 1 to 7 days (Carbon and Ditye, 2011, Carbon et al., 2007).

One of the curious aspects of these latter studies of long-term after-effects is that these were found despite the fact that subjects continued to be exposed to other faces in the course of their daily routines. This contrasts with the study of effects over several seconds in the laboratory, in which the time between adaptation and the subject׳s response is filled by either a blank interval of varying duration (Leopold et al., 2001) or merely by prolonging the duration of the test stimulus (Rhodes et al., 2007). Although there are ‘de-adaptation’ studies that employ a strategy of using an opposing stimulus to generate after-effects to counter-act those from the first stimulus (Mesik et al., 2013), it is not known whether even brief introduction of other face stimuli in the period between the adaptor and test stimuli affects the dynamics of the face after-effect. However, notwithstanding the findings from studies of long-term after-effects (Carbon and Ditye, 2011, Carbon et al., 2007), it is plausible that the effects of adaptation to one face would be altered and possibly reduced by viewing of another face. The goal of this study was to test the hypothesis that face identity after-effects would be reduced by exposure to other face stimuli.

Section snippets

Experiment 1

In our first experiment, we used the ‘perceptual bias’ method, in which ambiguous test stimuli are created by morphing between a pair of faces. After adapting to one of these two faces, subjects usually show a repulsive after-effect, in that they are more likely to respond that an ambiguous test stimulus looks more like the other face of the pair. We asked whether adaptation to the first face would be reduced if the subject was briefly exposed to the second face of the pair, just before the

Discussion

In Experiment 1, brief exposure of the second face of a pair in a perceptual bias paradigm did not decrease the after-effect generated by the first face. However, an attractive after-effect generated by the interfering face may have masked a decline in the repulsive after-effect from the adapting face. In Experiment 2, we eliminated this confound and explored the effect of variable delay periods. We found, first, that after-effects decline rapidly by about 50% between 300 ms and 1650 ms and that

Subjects

Twelve participants (9 female) took part in Experiment 1, with a mean age of 23.6 years (SD=4.1; range 18 to 32). All subjects had normal or corrected-to-normal vision, no history of psychiatric or neurological disease, and were naive to the purpose of the experiment. The protocols were approved by the institutional review boards of Vancouver General Hospital and the University of British Columbia, and written informed consent was obtained for all subjects in accordance with The Code of Ethics

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by NSERC Discovery Grant RGPIN 355879-08 and presented at the annual meeting of the Vision Sciences Society in Naples, FL, May 2013. JB was supported by a Canada Research Chair and the Marianne Koerner Chair in Brain Diseases.

References (36)

  • F.A. Verstraten et al.

    Recovery from motion adaptation is delayed by successively presented orthogonal motion

    Vis. Res.

    (1994)
  • J.M. Wolfe

    Short test flashes produce large tilt aftereffects

    Vis. Res.

    (1984)
  • J. Bales et al.

    The after-effect of the perception of curved lines

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1935)
  • C.-C. Carbon et al.

    Sustained effects of adaptation on the perception of familiar faces

    J. Exp. Psychol.: Hum. Percept. Perform.

    (2011)
  • C.-C. Carbon et al.

    Adaptation effects of highly familiar faces: Immediate and long lasting

    Mem. Cognit.

    (2007)
  • C.J. Fox et al.

    It doesn׳t matter how you feel. The facial identity aftereffect is invariant to changes in facial expression

    J Vis

    (2008)
  • L. Ganz

    Is the figural aftereffect an aftereffect? A review of its intensity, onset, decay, and transfer characteristics

    Psychol. Bull.

    (1966)
  • J.J. Gibson

    Adaptation, after-effect and contrast in the perception of curved lines

    J. Exp. Psychol.

    (1933)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text