Elsevier

Brain and Cognition

Volume 111, February 2017, Pages 51-62
Brain and Cognition

Adolescent neural response to reward is related to participant sex and task motivation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2016.10.003Get rights and content

Highlights

  • Boys show more robust activation of nucleus accumbens during reward processing.

  • Task motivation partially mediates sex differences in nucleus accumbens activity.

  • Boys make a higher percentage of risky selections on a risky decision-making task.

  • Boys report higher motivation to earn money; girls report higher state anxiety.

Abstract

Risky decision making is prominent during adolescence, perhaps contributed to by heightened sensation seeking and ongoing maturation of reward and dopamine systems in the brain, which are, in part, modulated by sex hormones. In this study, we examined sex differences in the neural substrates of reward sensitivity during a risky decision-making task and hypothesized that compared with girls, boys would show heightened brain activation in reward-relevant regions, particularly the nucleus accumbens, during reward receipt. Further, we hypothesized that testosterone and estradiol levels would mediate this sex difference. Moreover, we predicted boys would make more risky choices on the task. While boys showed increased nucleus accumbens blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response relative to girls, sex hormones did not mediate this effect. As predicted, boys made a higher percentage of risky decisions during the task. Interestingly, boys also self-reported more motivation to perform well and earn money on the task, while girls self-reported higher state anxiety prior to the scan session. Motivation to earn money partially mediated the effect of sex on nucleus accumbens activity during reward. Previous research shows that increased motivation and salience of reinforcers is linked with more robust striatal BOLD response, therefore psychosocial factors, in addition to sex, may play an important role in reward sensitivity. Elucidating neurobiological mechanisms that support adolescent sex differences in risky decision making has important implications for understanding individual differences that lead to advantageous and adverse behaviors that affect health outcomes.

Introduction

Following perinatal neural organization, adolescence marks a second wave of plasticity, during which numerous behavioral, social, and physiological changes occur that act to re-organize and activate the brain (Spear, 2013). This extended brain plasticity can be viewed as a double-edged sword, serving to augment vulnerability to biological and psychological insult, as well as support healthy neurodevelopment (Telzer, 2016). Processing of rewarding stimuli is particularly relevant during the adolescent period, given the rise in sensation seeking, which may contribute to increased reward sensitivity and risk taking in some youth (Romer & Hennessy, 2007). Dysregulated reward processing has been linked with affective and substance use disorders, the incidence of which increase substantially during adolescence (Davey et al., 2008, Ernst et al., 2006, Fairchild, 2011, MacPherson et al., 2010). As such, elucidating the neural mechanisms underlying adolescent reward sensitivity may help in promoting beneficial, rather than adverse, neuroplastic change.

Psychobiological models of adolescent risk taking posit an imbalance between reward processing and self-control, mirrored by enhanced functional activation of reward-sensitive regions (i.e. striatum, including nucleus accumbens) and diminished activation of self-regulatory brain regions (i.e. medial prefrontal cortex), which drives risk taking via inefficient regulation of reward-sensitive brain regions by self-regulatory regions (Casey, 2015, Ernst, 2014, Smith et al., 2013, Somerville et al., 2010). However, there is a paucity of data showing a direct relationship between reward sensitivity and risk taking (Braams et al., 2016, Braams et al., 2015, Galvan et al., 2006, van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2014, van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2015, Vorobyev et al., 2015), likely because there is substantial individual variability in reward sensitivity (Bjork and Pardini, 2015, Braams et al., 2015, Chick, 2015, Cservenka et al., 2012). Some of this variability may be due to individual differences in personality traits, such as sensation seeking (Cservenka et al., 2012, van Duijvenvoorde et al., 2014) and impulsivity (Forbes et al., 2009, Piray et al., 2015). Moreover, the link between reward sensitivity and risk taking may be partly explained by pubertal influences (Forbes et al., 2010, Urosevic et al., 2014), given that puberty has been shown to correlate with sensation seeking (Forbes and Dahl, 2010, Martin et al., 2002, Martin et al., 2006, Steinberg, 2004, Steinberg et al., 2008), reward sensitivity (Urosevic et al., 2014) and nucleus accumbens activity in response to rewards (Braams et al., 2015). Indeed, there is evidence that pubertal increases in sensation seeking predict real-world risky behavior, such as substance use (Kirillova et al., 2001, Martin et al., 2002).

Gonadal hormones, which are re-activated at the onset of puberty, have also been linked to reward processing. Previous work in adolescents showed a positive association between striatal activity in response to reward and endogenous levels of testosterone (Braams et al., 2015, Op de Macks et al., 2011) and estradiol (Op de Macks et al., 2011) in both males and females. Moreover, sex hormone levels have been positively associated with risk-taking behavior in adolescence (de Water et al., 2013, Martin et al., 1999, Peper et al., 2013, Peters et al., 2015, Vermeersch et al., 2008a, Vermeersch et al., 2008b). In studies that compared boys and girls directly, there is more evidence of a positive relationship between sex hormones and risky behavior in boys relative to girls (de Water et al., 2013, Peper et al., 2015, Peters et al., 2015), or compared to evidence indicating no sex difference (Peper, Koolschijn, & Crone, 2013). In young adults, sex hormone levels have been shown to predict risky behavior in both sexes to the same degree (Braams et al., 2016, Mehta et al., 2015, Nguyen et al., 2016, Stanton et al., 2011). The majority of this research supports a link between testosterone and risk taking (Braams et al., 2016, de Water et al., 2013, Martin et al., 1999, Mehta et al., 2015, Nguyen et al., 2016, Peper et al., 2015, Peper et al., 2013, Peters et al., 2015, Stanton et al., 2011, Vermeersch et al., 2008b), while a subset of studies also support a positive association between estradiol and risk taking (de Water et al., 2013, Martin et al., 1999, Peper et al., 2015, Vermeersch et al., 2008a). Only two studies have examined the relationship between reward sensitivity, as indexed by nucleus accumbens activity, sex hormones and risk taking (Braams et al., 2015, Braams et al., 2016). One of these studies reported that puberty, testosterone and risk taking explained nucleus accumbens activation during a gambling game in both males and females (Braams et al., 2015). The second study indicated that testosterone levels, but not nucleus accumbens activation during the same gambling task, predicted risky behavior, as indexed by self-reported alcohol use, two years later in males and females (Braams et al., 2016). The mechanism linking sex hormones, reward sensitivity and risk taking remains to be fully elucidated; however, the extant literature suggests that both testosterone and estradiol may be important in explaining risk-taking behavior during adolescence, particularly in boys.

Intriguingly, sex differences in striatal reactivity during reward processing have not been reported or examined in previous studies of adolescents (Braams et al., 2015, Braams et al., 2016, Forbes et al., 2010, Op de Macks et al., 2011). This is somewhat surprising, given the presence of sex differences in pubertal maturation, sex hormone levels (Tanner & Whitehouse, 1976), prefrontal cortical maturation (on average, girls mature approximately two years earlier than boys) (Lenroot et al., 2007) and sensation seeking (on average, boys report more sensation seeking than girls) (Romer and Hennessy, 2007, Steinberg et al., 2008, Zuckerman and Kuhlman, 2000). Thus, sex may be an important variable to consider for understanding individual differences in reward sensitivity and risk taking during adolescence. Indeed, one of the primary neurotransmitters involved in reward processing - dopamine (Berridge & Kringelbach, 2008) - develops in a sexually dimorphic manner during adolescence. Studies in rodents demonstrate enhanced dopamine release in females compared to males due to elevations in estradiol levels during puberty (Di Paolo et al., 1985, Sarvari et al., 2014, Xiao and Becker, 1994). In contrast, testosterone metabolites have been shown to mediate reward response following direct administration into the nucleus accumbens, which may be mediated by binding at γ-Aminobutyric acid (GABA) (Frye, Park, Tanaka, Rosellini, & Svare, 2001) and dopamine (Mhillaj et al., 2015) receptors. Additionally, both sex hormones have been shown to influence sensation seeking in adolescence (Kerschbaum et al., 2006, Vermeersch et al., 2009), indicating a role for sex hormones in dopamine activity and sensation seeking. Thus, examining the influence of sensation seeking and sex hormones on potential sex differences in reward sensitivity may inform psychobiological models of risk taking in adolescence.

The current study adds to this literature by examining sex differences in reward processing in a large sample of healthy adolescents, as well as the potentially mediating influence of sex hormones on observed sex differences. We hypothesized boys would show increased blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) response in the striatum, including nucleus accumbens, during reward receipt feedback, as well as heightened risky behavior during a risky decision-making task, compared to girls. These hypotheses were based on research showing higher sensation seeking in adolescent boys (Romer and Hennessy, 2007, Steinberg et al., 2008) and delayed prefrontal gray matter maturation in boys, compared to age-matched girls (Lenroot et al., 2007). We also predicted testosterone and estradiol would mediate sex differences in nucleus accumbens BOLD response, given their important role in pubertal development, sensation seeking and in modulating reward-relevant brain regions (Braams et al., 2015, Di Paolo et al., 1985, Frye et al., 2001, Op de Macks et al., 2011, Sarvari et al., 2014, Xiao and Becker, 1994).

Section snippets

Participant screening and exclusionary criteria

Participants underwent comprehensive structured interviews by trained research assistants to determine eligibility. Youth and parents completed separate structured telephone interviews that included the Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children Predictive Scales (Lucas et al., 2001), the Family History Assessment Module (Rice et al., 1995), and the Brief Lifetime version of the Customary Drinking and Drug Use Record (Brown et al., 1998). Exclusionary criteria included current diagnosis of

Participant characteristics and task behavior

Details on participant characteristics can be found in Table 1. Boys and girls did not differ in age, SES, IQ, sensation seeking, or general quality of sleep. Two-run averaged RMS head movement values exceeded 1.5 mm for five participants that were subsequently excluded from further analyses; RMS was not statistically different between boys and girls of the remaining sample. Sex hormone levels were not normally distributed and underwent log transformation. Boys had statistically greater serum

Discussion

In this study, sex differences in brain response to reward were observed in a large and carefully matched sample of adolescent boys and girls. Specifically, BOLD response following notification of receipt of monetary rewards was higher in males, relative to females, in several reward-relevant brain regions (Liu et al., 2011, Mohr et al., 2010), including the nucleus accumbens. Although testosterone was positively related with nucleus accumbens BOLD response, it did not mediate the relationship

Conclusions

In sum, sex differences in BOLD activity during reward processing were observed in a large sample of healthy adolescents. Regions related to reward processing, including nucleus accumbens (Liu et al., 2011, Mohr et al., 2010), were recruited more robustly in males during reward trials. Sex hormones did not mediate the effect of sex on nucleus accumbens activation even though testosterone was positively correlated with activation of this region, indicating that sex may be a stronger predictor of

Funding

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health [AA017664 to B.J.N., AA2368801 to G.A.]; and the American Psychological Association [APA 110415 to G.A.]. Funding sources had no involvement in study design, collection, analysis or interpretation of the data and no involvement in writing or deciding to publish this manuscript.

Acknowledgements

Members of the Developmental Brain Imaging Lab at OHSU are thanked for their efforts in data collection.

References (97)

  • M. Ernst et al.

    Choice selection and reward anticipation: An fMRI study

    Neuropsychologia

    (2004)
  • G. Fairchild

    The developmental psychopathology of motivation in adolescence

    Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • E.E. Forbes et al.

    Pubertal development and behavior: Hormonal activation of social and motivational tendencies

    Brain and Cognition

    (2010)
  • E.E. Forbes et al.

    PER2 rs2304672 polymorphism moderates circadian-relevant reward circuitry activity in adolescents

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2012)
  • C.A. Frye et al.

    The testosterone metabolite and neurosteroid 3alpha-androstanediol may mediate the effects of testosterone on conditioned place preference

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2001)
  • B.P. Hasler et al.

    Weekend-weekday advances in sleep timing are associated with altered reward-related brain function in healthy adolescents

    Biological Psychology

    (2012)
  • B.P. Hasler et al.

    An altered neural response to reward may contribute to alcohol problems among late adolescents with an evening chronotype

    Psychiatry Research

    (2013)
  • S.M. Holm et al.

    Reward-related brain function and sleep in pre/early pubertal and mid/late pubertal adolescents

    Journal of Adolescent Health

    (2009)
  • S.A. Jones et al.

    Binge drinking impacts dorsal striatal response during decision making in adolescents

    NeuroImage

    (2016)
  • P. Kumar et al.

    Differential effects of acute stress on anticipatory and consummatory phases of reward processing

    Neuroscience

    (2014)
  • R.K. Lenroot et al.

    Sexual dimorphism of brain developmental trajectories during childhood and adolescence

    NeuroImage

    (2007)
  • X. Liu et al.

    Common and distinct networks underlying reward valence and processing stages: A meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies

    Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews

    (2011)
  • C.P. Lucas et al.

    The DISC Predictive Scales (DPS): Efficiently screening for diagnoses

    Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2001)
  • C.A. Martin et al.

    Sensation seeking, puberty, and nicotine, alcohol, and marijuana use in adolescence

    Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry

    (2002)
  • P.H. Mehta et al.

    Testosterone and cortisol jointly modulate risk-taking

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2015)
  • T.V. Nguyen et al.

    A testosterone-related structural brain phenotype predicts aggressive behavior from childhood to adulthood

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2016)
  • R.C. Oldfield

    The assessment and analysis of handedness: The Edinburgh inventory

    Neuropsychologia

    (1971)
  • Z.A. Op de Macks et al.

    Testosterone levels correspond with increased ventral striatum activation in response to monetary rewards in adolescents

    Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2011)
  • S. Peters et al.

    The link between testosterone and amygdala-orbitofrontal cortex connectivity in adolescent alcohol use

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2015)
  • M. Sarvari et al.

    Estradiol and isotype-selective estrogen receptor agonists modulate the mesocortical dopaminergic system in gonadectomized female rats

    Brain Research

    (2014)
  • K. Siegeler et al.

    Early social instability affects plasma testosterone during adolescence but does not alter reproductive capacity or measures of stress later in life

    Physiology & Behavior

    (2013)
  • Smith et al.

    Impact of socio-emotional context, brain development, and pubertal maturation on adolescent risk-taking

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2013)
  • L.H. Somerville et al.

    A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues

    Brain and Cognition

    (2010)
  • S.J. Stanton et al.

    Testosterone is positively associated with risk taking in the Iowa Gambling Task

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2011)
  • V.R. Steele et al.

    Neuroimaging measures of error-processing: Extracting reliable signals from event-related potentials and functional magnetic resonance imaging

    NeuroImage

    (2016)
  • E.H. Telzer

    Dopaminergic reward sensitivity can promote adolescent health: A new perspective on the mechanism of ventral striatum activation

    Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience

    (2016)
  • E.H. Telzer et al.

    The effects of poor quality sleep on brain function and risk taking in adolescence

    NeuroImage

    (2013)
  • S. Urosevic et al.

    Pubertal status associations with reward and threat sensitivities and subcortical brain volumes during adolescence

    Brain and Cognition

    (2014)
  • L. van der Meij et al.

    Testosterone responses to competition: The opponent’s psychological state makes it challenging

    Biological Psychology

    (2010)
  • A.C. van Duijvenvoorde et al.

    A cross-sectional and longitudinal analysis of reward-related brain activation: Effects of age, pubertal stage, and reward sensitivity

    Brain and Cognition

    (2014)
  • H. Vermeersch et al.

    Estradiol, testosterone, differential association and aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent girls

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2008)
  • H. Vermeersch et al.

    The role of testosterone in aggressive and non-aggressive risk-taking in adolescent boys

    Hormones and Behavior

    (2008)
  • H. Vermeersch et al.

    The relationship between sex steroid hormones and behavioural inhibition, (BIS) and behavioural activation (BAS) in adolescent boys and girls

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2009)
  • L. Xiao et al.

    Quantitative microdialysis determination of extracellular striatal dopamine concentration in male and female rats: Effects of estrous cycle and gonadectomy

    Neuroscience Letters

    (1994)
  • S. Zilioli et al.

    Testosterone across successive competitions: Evidence for a ’winner effect’ in humans?

    Psychoneuroendocrinology

    (2014)
  • C.F. Zink et al.

    Human striatal responses to monetary reward depend on saliency

    Neuron

    (2004)
  • J. Bayer et al.

    Differential modulation of activity related to the anticipation of monetary gains and losses across the menstrual cycle

    European Journal of Neuroscience

    (2013)
  • K.C. Berridge et al.

    Affective neuroscience of pleasure: Reward in humans and animals

    Psychopharmacology (Berl)

    (2008)
  • Cited by (0)

    View full text