Elsevier

Vision Research

Volume 11, Issue 8, August 1971, Pages 833-840
Vision Research

The tilt after-effect: A fresh look

https://doi.org/10.1016/0042-6989(71)90005-8Get rights and content

Abstract

A high contract grating was used to induce the tilt after-effect. It was found to occur only close to the vertical and horizontal orientations. The interocular transfer of this effect is complete. Judgement of the vertical and horizontal orientation as measured by determining the Standard Deviation at a number of orientations is much more precise even when the cue of gravity is removed. All the characteristics of the tilt after-effect cannot be accounted for by adaption of neurones selectively sensitive to orientation, although they may well be involved.

Résumé

Une grille très fine a été utilisée pour produire une inclinaison par répercussion. On a trouvé que cet effet se produisait seulement près des orientations verticale et horizontale. Le transfert interoculaire de cet effet est complet. Le jugement de l'orientation verticale et horizontale, mesurée en déterminant la Dévìation Standard à un nombre d'orientations, est beaucoup plus précis même quand l'index de gravité est écarté. Toutes les caractéristiques de l'effet d'inclinaison qui suit, ne peuvent pas etre expliquées par l'adaptation des neurone sélectivement sensibles à l'orientation, quoiqu'ils pourraient bien être impliqués.

Zusammenfassung

Man verwendete ein stark zusammenziehendes Beugungsgitter zur Erzeugung der Kipp-Nachwirkung. Man fand, dass diese nur nahe den vertikalen und horizontalen Orientationen auftrat. Die zwischen den Augen stattfindende Wirkung dieses Effektes ist komplett. Beurteilung der vertikalen und horizontalen Orientierung bemessen nach der Bestimmung der Standardabweichung einer Reihe von Orientationen ist viel präziser, selbst nach Entfernung des Gravitationssignals. Nicht alle charakteristischen Merkmale der Kipp-Nachwirkung können durch Anpassung von Neuronen, die gegenüber Orientierung selektiv sensitiv sind, erklärt werden, obwohl es leicht möglich ist, dass sie etwas damit zu tun haben.

Реферат

Для пoлyчeния пocлeдeйcтвня oт нaклoнa иcпoльзoвaлacь peшeткa выcoкoй cзимaeмocти. Этo пocлeдeйcтвиe cкaзывaлocь тoлькo пpи opиeнтиpoвкaч близкич к вepтикaльнoй и гopизoнтaльнoй. Bнyтpиглaзнaя пepeдaчa зтoгo пocлeдeйcтвия пoлнaя. Cyждeниe o вepтикaльнoй и гopизoнтaльиoй opнeитиpoвкe, измepeннoй пyтeм oпpeдeлeния cтaндapтнoгo oтклoнeния пpи pядe opиeнтиpoвoк являeтcя eщe бoлee тoчным, кoгдa ycтpaняeтcя фaктop cилы тяжecти. Xapaктepиcтики пocлeдcтвия oт нaклoнa нe мoгyт быть цeликoм oтнeceны зa cчeт ceлeктивнoй aдaптaции чyвcтвитeльныч к opиeнтиpoвкe нeйpoнoв, нo oии мoгyт итpaть знaчитeльнyю poль.

References (20)

  • C. Blakemore et al.

    On the existence in the human visual system of neurons selectively sensitive to the orientation and size of retinal images

    J. Physiol

    (1969)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    The angular selectivity of visual cortical cells to moving gratings

    J. Physiol

    (1968)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    The spatial selectivity of the visual cells of the cat

    J. Physiol

    (1969)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    The spatial selectivity of visual cells of the cat and squirrel monkey

    J. Physiol

    (1969)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    Orientational selectivity of the human visual system

    J. Physiol

    (1966)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    The effect of orientation on the visual resolution of gratings

    J. Physiol

    (1966)
  • F.W. Campbell et al.

    Electrophysiological evidence for the existence of orientation and size detectors in the human visual system

    J. Physioi

    (1970)
  • J.J. Gibson

    Adaptation, after-effects, and contrast in the perception of curved lines

    J. exp. Psychol

    (1933)
  • J.J. Gibson et al.

    Adaptation, after effect, and contrast in the perception of tilted lines—I. Quantitative studies

    J. exp. Psychol

    (1937)
  • I.P. Howard et al.

    Human Spatial Orientation

    (1966)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.

Cited by (120)

  • Interaction of contexts in context-dependent orientation estimation

    2020, Vision Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Still, this discrepancy should probably not be considered as a strong argument against the theory, because orientations that are sufficiently far from the reference are obviously not suited to serve as good reference cues (and hence deserve a lower weight). More extensive modeling is outside the scope of this work, but TAE may indeed be linear with small orientation differences (Campbell & Maffei, 1971; Magnussen & Kurtenbach, 1980). Overall, the theory suggests how different reference cues are combined, which can account for the TAE and the TI.

  • What is a preattentive feature?

    2019, Current Opinion in Psychology
    Citation Excerpt :

    It is tempting to think that features should produce featural aftereffects. After all, for instance, orientation is a feature dimension and there are orientation (tilt) aftereffects [76,77]. The same is true for size, motion, and others [78].

  • Opposite Effects of Recent History on Perception and Decision

    2017, Current Biology
    Citation Excerpt :

    This novel dissociation of serial dependencies in perception and decision stands in opposition to previous reports of positive perceptual serial dependence [1–3, 15, 16] and highlights the importance of studying effects of sensory history on perceptual and post-perceptual processes in separation. The negative perceptual bias in experiments 2 and 3 was spatially specific and resembles the well-studied negative tilt-aftereffect in orientation perception [6–13], which is part of a larger family of negative adaptation effects [17–21]. It is widely assumed that adaptation serves to optimize visual processing [22–24] and maximizes sensitivity to change in the adapted feature dimension [25–27].

  • Tilt aftereffect due to adaptation to natural stimuli

    2015, Vision Research
    Citation Excerpt :

    Such studies are typically performed using synthetic patterns. For example, research about the tilt aftereffect (TAE), which is the change in the perceived orientation for stimuli near the adaptor orientation, is typically performed using synthetic lines (Gibson & Radner, 1937), gratings (Campbell & Maffei, 1971; Mitchell & Muir, 1976), or Gabor patches (Knapen, Rolfs, Wexler, & Cavanagh, 2010). However, natural vision is more complex, and the visual system operates differently for synthetic and natural input (Alam, Vilankar, Field, & Chandler, 2014; Carandini et al., 2005; Olshausen & Field, 2005).

View all citing articles on Scopus
1

On leave from Laboratorio di Neurofisiolgia del C.N.R., Pisa, Italy.

View full text