Skip to main content
Log in

Assessment of Weight/Shape Implicit Bias Related to Attractiveness, Fear, and Disgust

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) suggests that a positive bias toward thinness, rather than a negative bias toward fatness, might underlie implicitly assessed evaluative responding regarding body image. Because these results contrast with reports of widespread anti-fat attitudes, we aimed to clarify the nature of implicitly assessed evaluative body image responding by using stimuli more central to body image concerns. Seventy-five undergraduates (65.8 % female) completed two IRAPs to examine three aims: (1) create IRAPs sensitive to histories of self-oriented anti-fat responding by using attractiveness-, fear-, and disgust-based stimulus relations; (2) evaluate responses to the different types of stimulus relations on the IRAPs separately and compare them to each other; and (3) examine correlations among IRAP responding and self-report measures of disgust, fear of fatness, and eating pathology. Results showed that participants demonstrated a self-thin-attractive relational bias. Unexpectedly, respondents also showed a self-fat-attractive relational bias and did not display self-fat-disgusting or self-fat-fear responding. IRAP effects for self-thin-attractive stimulus relations were larger than those for self-fat-attractive stimulus relations, suggesting the pro-self bias on these two trial types was larger when relating to oneself as being thin than when relating to oneself as being overweight. Correlations with self-report measures were nonsignificant. Results are discussed in the context of gaining a better understanding of implicit evaluative body image responding and contextual variables that influence IRAP responding.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Throughout this manuscript, we use the terms bias, relational bias, and implicit bias to refer to relational responses that are emitted with high degrees of speed and/or accuracy because the specific stimulus relations involved in the responses have been derived or directly reinforced frequently in the past. In this way we do not use these terms to refer to the influence of unobserved mental constructs. For instance, an “implicit anti-fat relational bias” suggests that functionally similar relational responses involving negative responding toward overweight stimuli (e.g., being overweight is bad) are well-practiced, cohere with one’s relational history, and are emitted with relative ease and automaticity.

References

  • Anselmi, P., Vianello, M., & Robusto, E. (2013). Preferring thin people does not imply derogating fat people: A Rasch analysis of the implicit weight attitude. Obesity, 21, 261–265. doi:10.1002/oby.20085.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bacon, L. (2010). Health at every size: The surprising truth about your weight (2nd ed.). Dallas, TX: BenBella Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Power, P., Hayden, E., Milne, R., & Stewart, I. (2006). Do you really know what you believe? Developing the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a direct measure of implicit beliefs. The Irish Psychologist, 32, 169–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Stewart, I., & Boles, S. (2010). A sketch of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) and the relational elaboration and coherence (REC) model. The Psychological Record, 60, 527–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berg, K. C., Peterson, C. B., Frazier, P., & Crow, S. C. (2012). Psychometric evaluation of the Eating Disorder Examination and Eating Disorder Inventory Questionnaire: A systematic review of the literature. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 428–438. doi:10.1002/eat.20931.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carels, R. A., & Musher-Eizenman, D. R. (2010). Individual differences and weight bias: Do people with an anti-fat bias have a pro-thin bias? Body Image, 7, 143–148. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2009.11.005.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Cassin, S. E., & von Ranson, K. M. (2005). Word lists for testing cognitive biases in eating disorders. European Eating Disorders Review, 13, 216–220. doi:10.1002/erv.607.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chambliss, H. O., Finley, C. E., & Blair, S. N. (2004). Attitudes toward obese individuals among exercise science students. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 36, 468–474. doi:10.1249/01.MSS.0000117115.94062.E4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dalley, S. E., & Buunk, A. P. (2011). The motivation to diet in young women: Fear is stronger than hope. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 672–680. doi:10.1002/ejsp.816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Houwer, J. (2002). The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: Strengths and limitations. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 33, 115–133. doi:10.1016/s0005-7916(02)00024-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Drake, C. E., Kellum, K. K., Wilson, K. G., Luoma, J. B., Weinstein, J. H., & Adams, C. H. (2010). Examining the implicit relational assessment procedure: Four preliminary studies. The Psychological Record, 60, 81–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durso, L. E., Latner, J. D., White, M. A., Masheb, R. M., Blomquist, K. K., Morgan, P. T., & Grilo, C. M. (2012). Internalized weight bias in obese patients with binge eating disorder: Associations with eating disturbances and psychological functioning. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45, 423–427. doi:10.1002/eat.20933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Espeset, E., Gulliksen, K. S., Nordbø, R. H., Skårderud, F., & Holte, A. (2012). The link between negative emotions and eating disorder behaviour in patients with anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 20, 451–460. doi:10.1002/erv.2183.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fairburn, C. G., & Beglin, S. J. (2008). Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (6.0). In C. G. Fairburn (Ed.), Cognitive behavior therapy and eating disorders. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Hussey, I., & Graddy, J. (2016). Exploring the behavioral dynamics of the implicit relational assessment procedure: The impact of three types of introductory rules. Psychological Record. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s40732-016-0173-4

  • Goldfarb, L. A., Dykens, E. M., & Gerrard, M. (1986). The Goldfarb Fear of Fat Scale. Journal of Personality Assessment, 49, 329–332. doi:10.1207/s15327752jpa4903_21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Golijani-Moghaddam, N., Hart, A., & Dawson, D. L. (2013). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure: Emerging reliability and validity data. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 2, 105–119. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The Implicit Association Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 1464–1480. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B., & Banaji, M. (2003). Understanding and using the Implicit Association Test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197–216. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York, NY: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Holtgraves, T. (2004). Social desirability and self-reports: Testing models of socially desirable responding. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 161–172. doi:10.1177/0146167203259930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2011). On the formation and persistence of implicit attitudes: New evidence from the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP). The Psychological Record, 61, 391–410.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Vahey, N. (2012). Holding on to our functional roots when exploring new intellectual islands: A voyage through implicit cognition research. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 1, 17–38. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2012.09.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2015a). From relational frame theory to implicit attitudes and back again: Clarifying the link between RFT and IRAP research. Current Opinion in Psychology, 2, 11–15. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2014.12.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Daly, T., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015b). Life is good, but death ain’t bad either: Counter-intuitive implicit biases to death in a normative population. The Psychological Record. doi:10.1007/s40732-015-0142-3.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Ní Mhaoileoin, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Ohtsuki, T., Kishita, N., Hughes, S., & Murphy, C. (2016). The IRAP is non-relative but not a-contextual: Changes to the contrast category influence men’s de-humanization of women. The Psychological Record. doi:10.1007/s40732-016-0171-6.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hussey, I., Thompson, M., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2015c). Interpreting and inverting with less cursing: A guide to interpreting IRAP data. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 157–162. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2015.05.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juarascio, A. S., Forman, E. M., Timko, C. A., Herbert, J. D., Butryn, M., & Lowe, M. (2011). Implicit internalization of the thin ideal as a predictor of increases in weight, body dissatisfaction, and disordered eating. Eating Behaviors, 12, 207–213. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2011.04.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lieberman, D. L., Tybur, J. M., & Latner, J. D. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, obesity stigma, and gender: Contamination psychology predicts weight bias for women, not men. Obesity, 20, 1803–1814. doi:10.1038/oby.2011.247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maroto Expósito, P., Hernández López, M., & Rodríguez Valverde, M. (2015). Assessment of implicit anti-fat and pro-slim attitudes in young women using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 15, 17–32.

    Google Scholar 

  • Matlin, M. W. (2004). Pollyanna principle. In R. Pohl (Ed.), Cognitive illusions: A handbook on fallacies and biases in thinking, judgement and memory (pp. 257–271). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McKenna, I. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2007). Testing the fake-ability of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP): The first study. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 7, 123–138.

  • Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2012). Developing an implicit measure of disgust propensity and disgust sensitivity: Examining the role of implicit disgust propensity and sensitivity in obsessive-compulsive tendencies. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43, 922–930.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nicholson, E., Hopkins-Doyle, A., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, R. A. P. (2014). Psychopathology, anxiety or attentional control: Determining the variables which predict IRAP performance. The Psychological Record, 64, 179–188. doi:10.1007/s40732-014-0020-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nolan, J., Murphy, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2013). Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure and body-weight bias: Influence of gender of participants and targets. The Psychological Record, 63, 467–488. doi:10.11133/j.tpr.2013.63.3.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Brien, K. S., Daníelsdóttir, S., Ólafsson, R. P., Hansdóttir, I., Fridjónsdóttir, T. G., & Jónsdóttir, H. (2013). The relationship between physical appearance concerns, disgust, and anti-fat prejudice. Body Image, 10, 619–623. doi:10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.07.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Toole, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Three chronometric indices of relational responding as predictors of performance on a brief intelligence test: The importance of relational flexibility. The Psychological Record, 59, 119–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olatunji, B. O., Cisler, J. M., Deacon, B. J., Connolly, K., & Lohr, J. M. (2007). The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale–Revised: Psychometric properties and specificity in relation to anxiety disorder symptoms. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 918–930. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.12.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parling, T., Cernvall, M., Stewart, I., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Ghaderi, A. (2012). Using the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure to compare implicit pro-thin/anti-fat attitudes of patients with anorexia nervosa and non-clinical controls. Eating Disorders, 20, 127–143. doi:10.1080/10640266.2012.654056.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, C. B., Crosby, R. D., Wonderlich, S. A., Joiner, T., Crow, S. J., Mitchell, J. E., … & le Grange, D. (2007). Psychometric properties of the eating disorder examination-questionnaire: Factor structure and internal consistency. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40(4), 386–389. doi:10.1002/eat.20373

  • Puhl, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity Research, 9, 788–805. doi:10.1038/oby.2001.108.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Puhl, R. M., & Heuer, C. A. (2009). The stigma of obesity: A review and update. Obesity, 17, 941–964. doi:10.1038/oby.208.636.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ritzert, T. R., Forsyth, J. P., Berghoff, C. R., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Nicholson, E. (2015). The impact of a cognitive defusion intervention on behavioral and psychological flexibility: An experimental evaluation in a spider fearful non-clinical sample. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 4, 112–120. doi:10.1016/j.jcbs.2015.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roddy, S., Stewart, I., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2009). Anti-fat, pro-thin, or both?: Using two reaction-time based measures to assess implicit attitudes to the thin and overweight. Journal of Health Psychology, 15, 416–425. doi:10.1177/1359105309350232.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roddy, S., Stewart, I., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2011). Facial reactions reveal that slim is good but fat is not bad: Implicit and explicit measures of body-size bias. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41, 688–694. doi:10.1002/ejsp.839.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rushford, N. (2006). Fear of gaining weight: Its validity as a visual analog scale in anorexia nervosa. European Eating Disorders Review, 14, 104–110. doi:10.1002/erv.682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. B., Chambliss, H. O. N., Brownell, K. D., Blair, S. N., & Billington, C. (2003). Weight bias among health professionals specializing in obesity. Obesity Research, 11, 1033–1039. doi:10.1038/oby.2003.142.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M. B., Vartanian, L. R., Nosek, B. A., & Brownell, K. D. (2006). The Influence of one’s own body weight on implicit and explicit anti-fat bias. Obesity, 14, 440–447. doi:10.1038/oby.2006.58.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, S. E., & Brown, J. D. (1988). Illusion and well-being: A social psychological perspective on mental health. Psychological Bulletin, 103, 193–210. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.103.2.193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, J. K., & Stice, E. (2001). Thin-ideal internalization: Mounting evidence for a new risk factor for body-image disturbance and eating pathology. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 181–183. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Timko, C. A., England, E. L., Herbert, J. D., & Forman, E. M. (2010). The Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure as a measure of self-esteem. The Psychological Record, 60, 679–698.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tomiyama, A. J., Finch, L. E., Belsky, A. C. I., Buss, J., Finley, C., Schwartz, M. B., & Daubenmier, J. (2015). Weight bias in 2001 versus 2013: Contradictory attitudes among obesity researches and health professionals. Obesity, 23, 46–53. doi:10.1002/oby.20910.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vahey, N. A., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Stewart, I. (2009). A first test of the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) as a measure of self-esteem: Irish prisoner groups and university students. The Psychological Record, 59, 371–388.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vahey, N., Boles, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2010). Measuring adolescents’ smoking-related social identity preferences with the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) for the first time: A starting point that explains later IRAP evolutions. The International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 10, 453–474.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vahey, N. A., Nicholson, E., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2015). A meta-analysis of criterion effects for the Implicit Relational Assessment Procedure (IRAP) in the clinical domain. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 48, 59–65. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.01.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Overveld, M., de Jong, P. J., & Peters, M. L. (2010). The Disgust Propensity and Sensitivity Scale–Revised: Its predictive value for avoidance behavior. Personality and Individual Differences, 49, 706–711. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2010.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woud, M. L., Anschutz, D. J., Van Strien, T., & Becker, E. S. (2011). Measuring thinspiration and fear of fat indirectly: A matter of approach and avoidance. Appetite, 56, 451–455. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.12.012.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author Note

We are thankful for Lauren Knauf, who helped with study administration; Jillian Montreuil, who helped with numerous literature searches for this article; and Dermot Barnes-Holmes, for his comments and suggestions related to this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Timothy R. Ritzert.

Ethics declarations

Funding

The authors have no funding to report.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki Declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Ritzert, T.R., Anderson, L.M., Reilly, E.E. et al. Assessment of Weight/Shape Implicit Bias Related to Attractiveness, Fear, and Disgust. Psychol Rec 66, 405–417 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0181-4

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-016-0181-4

Keywords

Navigation