Introduction

The Problem of Obesity

The prevalence of obesity has resulted in a global epidemic in recent times. Worldwide, in 2014, there were an estimated 1.9 billion overweight adults, 18 years and older, with over 600 million of these classed as obese [1]. In a large cohort of over 20,000 adult participants in the USA, the prevalence of overweight and obesity increased from 31.2 to 36.9 % and from 10.2 to 27.7 % from 1977 to 2009, respectively [2]. The obesity epidemic is affecting not only adults but also children, with the World Health Organization (WHO) estimating that in 2013, 42 million children younger than 5 years of age were either overweight or obese [1]. This is of a greater concern as childhood obesity tends to persist into adulthood and increases the risk and severity of obesity-associated diseases [36]. The WHO defines overweight and obesity as “abnormal or excessive fat accumulation that presents a risk to health” [1]. Body mass index (BMI; weight (kg) / [height (m)]2) is commonly used as a measure to classify overweight and obesity. In adults, the WHO classifies overweight as a BMI of greater than or equal to 25 kg/m2 and obesity as a BMI of greater than or equal to 30 kg/m2 although lower cut-points have been recommended in several other populations [1].

Obesity is not only a significant challenge to those burdened with the disease and its complications but also has a serious impact upon health-care systems and society as a whole. Some of the many comorbidities associated with obesity include type 2 diabetes mellitus [7], cardiovascular disease (CVD), several cancers [3], and depression [8]. Obesity and overweight have also been associated with increased fasting glucose levels, a reflection of insulin insensitivity [9]. Key drivers to obesity comorbidities such as insulin insensitivity are changes in the array of adipocytokines released by adipocytes and chronic inflammation.

Based on the first law of thermodynamics, weight gain occurs when the consumption of calories exceeds overall energy expenditure including physical activity. Excess energy is stored primarily in white adipose tissue as fat [10]. Major contributing factors to excess energy intake are high-fat calorie-dense foods and sugar-sweetened beverages combined with large portion size [10, 11]. The quantity and nutritional composition of food ingested are influenced by many factors including biological, psychological, and environmental factors [12].

Obesity and Sleep

In recent years, while the prevalence of obesity has increased, several studies have reported a decline in sleep duration [13]. This suggests that there may be a relationship between obesity and sleep duration, which could be due to the opportunities and demands of modern society, use of media, socioeconomic status, work schedules, family dynamics, culture, and/or mental health. The prevalence of very short sleep (<5 h) and short sleep (5–6 h) in the USA increased from 1.7 to 2.4 % and from 19.7 to 26.7 % from 1977 to 2009, respectively [2]. The prevalence for long sleep decreased from 11.6 to 7.8 % during the same time period [2]. Data regarding secular trends in sleep duration, however, are controversial, as there are many deficiencies in the available data and not all reports confirm a reduction in sleep duration over time.

Sleep, or the lack of it, is increasingly recognized as a risk factor for obesity. Sleep is important as it plays a significant role in the restorative process of the body and its energy metabolism. In sleep, the body requires less energy to support brain function, sympathetic activity, breathing, and circulation are reduced; and the core body temperature is lower so there is a 20-30 % decrease in basal metabolic rate. The lack of sleep may therefore hinder these processes and cause adverse effects, such as weight gain and obesity.

Epidemiological studies have linked short sleep duration with the development of obesity and other metabolic disorders such as type 2 diabetes mellitus. Weight gain and obesity associated with short sleep duration have been attributed to increased ghrelin levels and decreased leptin levels. Ghrelin is a stomach-derived hormone that signals hunger to the hypothalamus to initiate food intake. Ghrelin levels are lower in obesity as a compensatory mechanism but are high in conditions associated with excess food intake such as Prader-Willi syndrome. Low ghrelin levels have also been observed post gastric bypass surgery, suggesting that it has a key role in reduction of appetite and weight loss after the procedure. Leptin is released by adipocytes to signal sufficient energy stores to the hypothalamus. In obesity, leptin levels are higher, suggesting the presence of leptin resistance, which has been shown to be due to defective leptin transport to the central nervous system. Thus, high leptin levels, as occurring in obesity, are not physiologically as important as low leptin levels which signal energy deficit triggering increased food intake. When leptin levels are low and ghrelin is high, this results in an increased appetite. These changes are observed after calorie deficit as occurring during dieting and may explain why weight loss can be so difficult. Low leptin and high ghrelin levels associated with short sleep can induce an increase of daily energy consumption, subsequent weight gain and obesity.

In adults, habitual short (sleeping ≤6 h per night) and long (sleeping >9 h nightly) sleep durations have been recognized as behavioral risk factors for morbidity and mortality [14]. The association between short sleep duration and BMI has been reported by many epidemiological cross-sectional and prospective studies. Studies have shown a U-shaped relationship between habitual sleep duration and body weight in adults. Data from 1024 volunteers from the Wisconsin Sleep Cohort Study highlighted that those who slept less than 8 h per night (74.4 % of the sample) were more likely to be overweight [15]. The study also found that short sleep duration was associated with low leptin and high ghrelin levels. Independent of BMI, the study also observed 15.5 % lower leptin for habitual sleep of 5 h versus 8 h and 14.9 % higher ghrelin for nocturnal (polysomnographic) sleep of 5 h versus 8 h [15]. The changes in the levels of these hormones are equivalent to those observed with about a 1000 kcal daily energy deficit. More recently, a study analyzed self-reported data from the National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES) between 2005 and 2010. This study found an inverse linear relationship in the 13,742 study participants aged ≥20 years (median age of 46 years). The short sleepers, defined as sleep duration of ≤6 h, were heavier (approximately 1.7 kg/m2, SE 0.4) and had a larger waist circumference (3.4 cm, SE 1.0) than long sleepers, defined as ≥10 h [16]. Even small increases in BMI over time can be associated with increased risk of pre-diabetes and diabetes. Furthermore, such a change in BMI has a serious impact on health at a population level.

Interestingly, Nagai and colleagues found no association between sleep duration and risk of weight gain ≥5 kg (p for trend = 0.62) and obesity in their Japanese participants when they examined the long-term association between sleep duration, weight gain, and obesity [17]. However, they did observe a significantly increased risk of ≥5 kg weight gain (odds ratio [OR] 1.36, 95 % CI 1.09–1.70) in obese long sleepers (≥9 h of sleep). After acknowledging that this group was the eldest in the analysis (mean age of 61.9 years) and so may have included participants bedridden due to physical limitation, they further analyzed the data to exclude poor self-rated health, functional limitations, or history of disease. Even then, only obese long sleepers were found to be at significantly increased risk of weight gain (OR 1.41, 95 % CI 1.04–1.92).

An independent and inverse relationship was found more recently between sleep duration and visceral fat tissue accumulation. This was reported by Chaput and colleagues who carried out longitudinal analysis on 293 participants to investigate the relationship between change in sleep duration and long-term visceral adiposity alteration in adults [18]. They found that over the 6-year follow-up period, on average, participants gained 19.2 ± 37.3 cm2 in visceral fat tissue. They observed an independent association with 6 cm2 fewer visceral fat tissue gain over the follow-up period and a change in sleep duration from ≤6 to 7–8 h/day.

Short sleep duration, resulting in insufficient sleep, can cause neuroendocrine, metabolic, and behavioral adaptations. This, in combination with the abundance of freely available energy dense foods, can cause a greater energy imbalance and lead to weight gain and resultant obesity. Dietary and sleep behaviors have been a field of growing research interest. Using the NHANES data (n = 14,992), short sleepers (≤6 h of sleep) were found to not only to eat earlier in the day but also ate later in the day [19]. Most studies support the concept that restricting sleep increases food intake. However, more eating events were not reported in this NHANES analysis. Interestingly though, this analysis showed that a smaller proportion of short sleepers, compared to average sleepers (7–8 h of sleep), reported eating breakfast and dinner (p < 0.04). A higher percentage of short sleepers who reported having breakfast also reported having a snack (defined as eating episodes that were not main meals) before breakfast (p ≤ 0.004).

There are several deficiencies in population-based studies that need to be considered. The majority of the available studies have been cross-sectional with many not originally designed to examine the impact of sleep on health. There has been a lack of use of objective measures to determine sleep duration and quality in naturalistic environments. Often, sleep has been assessed by only a single question. Nevertheless, the body of evidence is highly compelling and is also supported by experimental human sleep laboratory and animal studies, which have identified some of the potential mechanisms.

While the epidemiological and prospective evidence continues to support the short sleep-obesity association, the focus of attention has increasingly switched to mechanistic explanations, which include energy intake/expenditure and feeding behaviors in response to sleep loss. Diet is difficult to quantify in large samples and usually relies upon subjective reports, which are prone to multiple biases/inaccuracies. Experimental evidence in tightly controlled settings has revealed important explanations.

Sleep Loss and Excess Energy Intake: Experimental Evidence

Detailed experimental work has provided a number of mechanistic clues concerning the relationship between sleep, metabolic disturbance, and obesity. The effects of sleep loss upon dietary behaviors has been a recent topic of interest. A number of studies offer explanations regarding how sleep loss can result in weight gain and obesity. Spaeth and colleagues have performed the largest randomized sleep restriction study to date, recruiting 225 adults aged 22–50 years [20]. Participants were randomized to either five nights of 4-h time in bed (0400–0800 hours) or the control condition of 10-h time in bed (2200–0800 hours). Those in the 5-day sleep restriction group gained 0.86 kg more weight compared to controls (p < 0.007). Furthermore, sleep-restricted individuals consumed 30 % additional calories per day compared to controls (p = 0.003). Specifically, the extra calories consumed were a result of more meals and calories during evening/early hours (2200–0359 hours). Moreover, calories consumed from fat between 2200 and 0359 hours was approximately 5 % higher compared to daytime (0800–1459 hours) and over 3 % more compared to 1500–2159 hours, p < 0.05. The same group, using the same study protocol, investigated differences between ethnic groups for energy intake with sleep restriction versus without. There was no evidence for differences between African-Americans and Caucasians for calorie intake. The findings, however, remained consistent for increased calorie consumption (about 24 %, p < 0.001) and fat intake (>1 %, p = 0.024) following sleep restriction in the total sample [21].

Portion size selection/consumption is one possible explanation for increased calorie intake observed in relation to acute total sleep deprivation compared to healthy sleep (8 h). This was recently explored by Hogenkamp and colleagues in 16 young, healthy men who were recruited to a randomized crossover study with two conditions: one night of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and one night of 8-h time in bed (monitored with polysomnography, PSG) separated by a 28-day washout period [22]. Subsequent to each condition, participants rated their hunger using a 10-cm visual analogue scale and completed a “portion size task.” This task included presentation of images on a laptop (seven meals and six snacks). Participants were asked to imagine that they could have the food that appeared in the image and identify portion size using arrow keys on the keyboard to either increase or decrease the portion size until it represented what they desired. The authors calculated the energy value for each rating, and the participants completed the task in both a fasted and non-fasted state. Subjective hunger ratings were confirmed to be higher following TSD compared to recommended sleep duration of 8 h, supporting previous findings. For all food items (meals and snacks) combined, average portion size when in a fasted state and sleep deprived (482 kcal) was significantly elevated compared to the 8-h sleep condition (422 kcal), p = 0.02. No significant differences were reported when the food items were stratified by meal/snack and fasted between sleep conditions. It is of interest that when participants were in a non-fasted state, there was an overall significant effect of combined food items when sleep deprived versus rested (399 and 356 kcal, respectively, p < 0.01) as well as snacks (364 and 313 kcal, respectively, p = 0.02). No differences were observed for meal items between sleep conditions in the non-fasted state. Similar findings, by the same group, have been reported in relation to food purchasing when provided with a fixed budget and TSD [23]. Limitations of the study are that TSD is not representative of societal sleep behaviors where sleep quantity is reduced or insufficient in a proportion of the population. Also, partial sleep loss that commonly occurs is likely to result in an accumulated sleep debt over time that will have an impact on metabolic regulation, cognitive function, and decision-making. Furthermore, desired portion sizes provide only an indication of appetite for the food item presented but do not quantify actual consumption. Finally, the results may not be generalizable to other populations (children, older adults, women) although the findings appear consistent in small samples of children [24] and adolescents [25, 26] with the effect of gender being recently explored.

Gender differences for metabolic regulation have been demonstrated in response to sleep restriction. Male and female volunteers (n = 27) with healthy weights aged 30–45 years, recruited to a randomized crossover study, were exposed to two sleep conditions (three nights of 4 or 9 h in bed) and provided with a controlled diet with fixed timings for meals [27]. The levels of two opposing gut hormones, ghrelin and glucagon-like peptide-1 (GLP-1), were altered in men but not in women following sleep restriction. Ghrelin, associated with hunger levels, increased with sleep restriction compared to controls. Conversely, GLP-1, associated with satiety and pancreatic beta cell insulin secretion, was reduced following sleep restriction. In combination, persistent metabolic hormone changes may promote the onset of type 2 diabetes mellitus and obesity through surplus energy intake. Based on this study, different mechanisms are likely according to gender although the underlying reasons for this are not yet clear.

The Psychology of Sleep and Over-feeding

While these recent findings that suggest a relationship between sleep loss and food intake beyond individual requirements may be partially explained by alterations in energy metabolism, psychological processes have also been implicated. For example, sleep loss has been previously shown to impair cognitive function [28] in a dose-dependent manner. Sleep loss in relation to decision-making and impulsivity has also been documented, although there is limited evidence regarding food selection/intake. Preliminary evidence has emerged from a recent study of 14 young (mean age of 22 years), healthy men [29]. In a randomized crossover study, participants underwent 24 h of total sleep deprivation (TSD) and one night of 8.5-h time in bed while being monitored by polysomnography (PSG), separated by a 4-week washout period. The morning after each condition, participants self-reported hunger levels and fasting blood samples were drawn for assessment of glucose concentration, followed by a cognitive task to determine inhibition (go/no-go task: the participant was presented with a series of non-food- and food-related words and instructed to press a button only when a non-food-related word was viewed) with the number of errors serving as the primary outcome measure. Main findings of this study included a 56 % increased number of errors on the inhibition task following TSD compared to the normal sleep condition (p < 0.05). Consistent with other research findings, subjective hunger (measured using a visual analogue scale) significantly increased from 2.7 cm following 8.5-h time in bed to 6.3 cm following TSD. Contrary to other studies [30], no significant differences were observed between the two conditions for glucose levels. While TSD does not reflect societal sleep behaviors, this initial evidence has revealed one possible mechanistic explanation of how sleep loss can skew energy balance.

Further advances have been recently made which support neuronal networks and responses to food stimuli following partial sleep loss. A study of 25 healthy weight men (n = 13) and women (n = 12) was randomized to undertake five nights of either 4 or 9 h time in bed [31]. At the end of the sleep condition, participants underwent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) in a fasted state and were presented with images of healthy and unhealthy food types. Neuronal response networks were assessed and compared between healthy and unhealthy food stimuli. Heightened activation of specific neuronal regions, associated with reward, was observed in response to unhealthy food images compared to healthy stimuli in participants who were sleep restricted. Specifically, the right insula had greater activation for unhealthy foods following sleep restriction, a brain region that mediates energy homeostasis and pleasure-seeking behaviors. These preliminary findings have aided a better mechanistic understanding of how sleep loss can affect neuronal networks and cognitive processes. More research in this area is, however, required to ensure full understanding of the processes involved in feeding behaviors in response to sleep loss. Moreover, these studies could be repeated to investigate the potential effect of energy expenditure on neuronal processes following manipulation of sleep loss.

Sleep Loss and Insufficient Energy Expenditure: Experimental Evidence

Positive energy occurs not only from excess energy intake but also from a lack of compensatory energy expenditure. While the evidence surrounding excess energy intake from experimental sleep manipulation studies continues to support a link between positive energy balance, it is important to consider both sides of the energy balance equation. Excess energy intake is acceptable if energy expenditure can counteract it and has been the recent focus of research attempting to complete our mechanistic understanding of the sleep-feeding-obesity relationship. Of the few studies that have examined sleep restriction and energy expenditure in adults, the findings are varied. One study confirmed increased calorie intake with sleep restriction (two thirds of habitual sleep duration) but found no change in energy expenditure [32]. Of note, energy expenditure was measured through utilization of accelerometry, which has been used and validated to estimate physical activity level rather than energy expenditure per se. Markwald and colleagues reported opposing evidence of energy expenditure measured by whole-room calorimetry [33]. An increase in total daily energy expenditure (about 5 %) was observed at the end of a five-night period of sleep restriction, designed to represent a typical working week where individuals have 5-h time in bed, compared to adequate sleep (9 h). Consistent with previous studies [34, 35], increased energy intake was observed in response to sleep restriction but the increase in energy expenditure was insufficient, resulting in positive energy and weight gain (0.82 kg) over just 5 days/nights.

The effect of disrupted sleep upon energy expenditure has also been explored in experimental studies. Using a randomized crossover design, Hursel and colleagues subjected 15 healthy, young men to two conditions: two nights of 8 h of undisturbed sleep and 8 h of disturbed sleep where participants received acoustic stimuli every hour while being monitored with PSG [36]. Energy expenditure was measured using a respiratory chamber where participants were housed throughout each sleep condition. Sleep quality was quantified using slow-wave sleep (SWS), rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, and total sleep time to derive sleep quality percentage. An inverse relationship was observed between total energy expenditure and sleep quality, where r = −0.57, p = 0.03. A possible explanation for this is that wakefulness, occurring from disrupted sleep, will enhance movement and therefore increase energy output. Furthermore, higher activity levels may promote better sleep quality.

Extended wakefulness may promote physical inactivity and provide increased opportunity to feed. Sleep loss clearly results in increases in energy intake, possibly to combat reduced energy levels arising from sleep deprivation. Sleep deprivation per se, results in shifted sleep-wake timings and thus challenges circadian rhythmicity, an area recently investigated in relation to energy balance and metabolic disturbance.

The Effects of a Challenged Circadian Clock on Metabolic Health

Wakefulness during night hours places a challenge upon the intrinsic biological (circadian) rhythm. Wakefulness when the circadian clock encourages sleep has been shown to result in serious, yet preventable, adverse health consequences. Shift workers serve as a comparable experimental model, and research has consistently verified a constellation of negative outcomes including elevated body mass index (BMI), deranged glucose metabolism, and increased prevalence of type 2 diabetes mellitus, cardiovascular disease, and metabolic syndrome [37, 38]. Night shift work is an extreme example of circadian challenge, and recent experimental work by McHill and colleagues, which mimicked night shift work, supports the theory of disrupted energy metabolism with circadian misalignment. The study, which recruited 14 young, healthy adults (eight females) to a 6-day inpatient stay and mimicked shift work, observed a number of significant findings [39]. The volunteers were monitored with wrist actigraphy for 1-week prior to laboratory attendance to verify sleep-wake patterns, sleep duration (about 8 h), and light exposure. Sleep opportunity was experimentally manipulated after day 3 of the inpatient stay when volunteers were awakened at their usual wake time, provided with a 2-h nap opportunity before the first simulated night shift (awake all night) and then given an 8-h sleep opportunity during day 4 which commenced 1-h after their usual wake time. A further two night shifts were scheduled with an 8-h daytime sleep opportunity. Multiple outcome measures were ascertained including energy expenditure and macronutrient utilization determined by whole-room indirect calorimetry and 24-h urine sampling. A series of blood samples were also obtained to determine levels of leptin, ghrelin, peptide tyrosine-tyrosine (PYY; a gut-derived satiety hormone), and pineal-derived melatonin. Subjective hunger was measured every 2 h using a visual analogue scale, and PSG was utilized to assess multiple sleep characteristics. Three meals per day were provided to meet energy balance needs, although there was no indication of what the meals contained or how individual energy requirements were determined. The main finding was the significant reduction in daily total energy expenditure, albeit minimal (about 3 %). Protein and carbohydrate utilization was significantly reduced on the second night shift only. Hunger levels decreased despite a significant reduction from baseline in leptin (about 41 %) and PYY (about 11 %) levels, which signal satiety through hypothalamic pathways. These findings have enhanced our understanding of energy expenditure in response to extreme circadian misalignment. However, circadian preference is not usually this extreme, with the exception of some sleep disorders (advance sleep phase delay, delayed sleep phase syndrome).

Circadian preference (chronotype), which is either late (night owls, usually prevalent in adolescents) or early (early bird/larks, more commonly observed in older adults), is likely to result in altered sleep-wake timings and has been the recent focus of study in relation to energy metabolism and energy intake. Our knowledge concerning sleep restriction and its relationship with energy intake has recently been extended with some groups exploring the association between sleep timing (a reflection of circadian preference) and food selection. A recent study assessed chronotype (verified by wrist actigraphy), BMI, and self-reported eating habits of 511 young adolescents [40]. Compared to morning types, evening chronotype was positively associated with BMI z-score (β = 0.51, p < 0.01). Poorer dietary habits, including increased frequency of eating unhealthy snacks and night-time caffeine consumption as well as inadequate daily intake of fruit and vegetables (p ≤ 0.01), were also observed in those with a later circadian preference. While it can be argued that circadian phase delay is more common in adolescents, there are consistent data in adults for metabolic disruption [41] as well as type 2 diabetes mellitus [42] and the metabolic syndrome [42].

From an evolutionary perspective, sleep timing per se, rather than sleep duration/loss, may be an important factor for optimum metabolic regulation and energy balance. For example, one study collected information on sleep-wake timings (sleep diary) and assessed delayed sleep timings relative to physical activity (waist-worn actigraphy) in 22 adults. After adjusting for sleep length, both later sleep (p = 0.013) and wake times (p = 0.005) were significantly associated with less time performing moderate-to-vigorous physical activity and increased sedentariness [43]. Given that both sleep loss and late chronotype have been linked to obesity and metabolic disturbance, it would be feasible to assume that if the two were combined, a greater effect would be observed. This was recently explored under tightly controlled laboratory conditions, subjecting 24 participants to 3 weeks of reduced sleep (5.6 h) paired with circadian disruption [44]. Standardized meals were provided, and regular blood sampling was performed to investigate levels of numerous metabolic hormones. While all participants were free of metabolic disease at baseline, during the sleep manipulation, three volunteers qualified for pre-diabetes based on post-prandial glucose levels. The levels of ghrelin were significantly increased during the sleep loss/circadian misalignment phase (p < 0.05), and leptin was lower (p = 0.05) compared to baseline levels. While normalized sleep was permitted for nine nights after sleep manipulation and those with pre-diabetes reverted back to healthy glucose levels, to disentangle the relationship, future studies could randomize individuals to either (1) continue with sleep loss and normalize the circadian rhythm or (2) continue with circadian misalignment with a recovery sleep opportunity. Identifying which sleep parameter has the greatest effect will better inform future strategies for targeting the treatment of those with metabolic disease. There are some preliminary data supporting improved weight loss outcomes in early chronotypes with earlier meal timings [20]. Further study is required, but combining lifestyle changes such as going to bed earlier and eating meals earlier may be simple yet effective behavior changes that are relatively easy to adhere to.

Napping: Good or Bad?

Sleep deprivation results in a higher propensity to nap during the day, although this behavior may also occur in the absence of sleep problems. The consequences/benefits of napping are unclear with heterogeneous findings [4547]. Habitual, frequent napping has been previously associated with an increased risk of type 2 diabetes mellitus after adjustment for multiple sleep factors that indicate insufficient nocturnal sleep [46]. Conversely, others have shown improvements in cognitive performance with napping, subsequent to acute total sleep loss in a laboratory setting [48].

It is not yet known if providing a nap opportunity can help to alleviate adverse metabolic hormone alterations that promote hunger, appetite, and satiety, in response to accumulated sleep debt. Furthermore, napping may help to improve subsequent hunger, appetite, and satiety levels observed with sleep insufficiency. Napping (2-h opportunity) in response to acute total sleep deprivation has been previously explored [49]. Contrary to previous evidence [29], no association was found between subjective hunger and 24 h of total sleep deprivation. Participants randomized to a 2-h nap opportunity condition did not report significant alterations in subjective hunger ratings. While this initial evidence is not promising, further investigation in this area is needed. Total sleep deprivation is unrepresentative of societal sleep behaviors; thus, protocols intended for chronic partial sleep restriction with and without napping opportunities are required. This will help to assess if napping is a potential approach for regulating energy intake, appetite, hunger, and metabolic hormones associated with feeding behaviors.

A consistent recommendation for improving health outcomes in short sleepers is to develop and apply a sleep extension intervention to improve nocturnal sleep. It may, however, be possible to achieve the same outcomes with napping as a means of overcoming hunger and over-eating, following inadequate sleep. This has not yet been examined but may provide an avenue for future research.

Conclusions

There is some preliminary evidence that extending sleep quantity in overweight, short sleepers is possible, with beneficial effects on energy balance [50] but additional studies are required. It is clear from emerging evidence that sleep plays a major role in the regulation of energy metabolism. Sleep loss clearly results in metabolic derangements, changes in appetite, hunger, and feeding, all of which may lead to surplus energy. While one function of sleep is to preserve energy, it is assumed that sleep loss, from additional wakefulness, results in increased energy expenditure although this has not been consistently confirmed. Conflicting suggestions include the proposal that inadequate sleep results in tiredness and may, in turn, lead to reduced motivation to engage in physical activity. Detailed evidence is needed for energy expenditure in response to sleep loss and circadian desynchronization. Experimental napping to reduce sleep debt may or may not improve energy metabolism, although this is still to be established.