Skip to main content
Log in

Social-Spatial Effects in Pricing: When and How Vertical Orientations Shape Processing of Price Comparisons

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Customer Needs and Solutions Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Marketers frequently compare a product’s sale price against its regular price to accentuate perceptions of value. The effectiveness of these price comparisons is predicated on consumers sufficiently processing the deal’s depth (i.e., the regular price − sale price differential). Although research indicates consumers can generally make such assessments, we document an exception when price comparisons are presented in a format that aligns with the vertical orientation of consumers. The correspondence between vertical price comparisons and the mindsets of individuals who value social hierarchies (i.e., who are vertically oriented) induces a sense of “fit” that reduces processing and leads to similar responses to the offer regardless of deal depth. This effect manifests regardless of whether vertical orientations are measured as a chronic state or temporarily primed. Supporting our theorizing, vertically oriented participants report paying less attention and being less motivated to process price comparisons. Theoretical implications of these findings are discussed as are suggestions for future research examining how social views may shape consumer processing of product attribute and point-of-purchase information.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. With covariates excluded, this interaction is marginally significant (F(1, 126) = 3.52, p = .06).

References

  1. Allard T, Griffin D (2017) Comparative price and the design of effective product communications. J Mark 81(5):16–29

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Alter AL (2013) The benefits of cognitive disfluency. Curr Dir Psychol Sci 22(6):437–442

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Alter AL , Oppenheimer DM., Epley N, Eyre R N. (2007) Overcoming intuition: Metacognitive difficulty activates analytic reasoning. J Exp Psychol Gen 136(4): 569–576

  4. Alter AL, Oppenheimer DM (2009) Uniting the tribes of fluency to form a metacognitive nation. Personal Soc Psychol Rev 13(3):219–235

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Ariely D, Gneezy U, Haruvy E (2018) Social norms and the price of zero. J Consum Psychol 28(2):180–191

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Barone MJ, Lyle KB, Winterich KP (2015) When deal depth doesn’t matter: How handedness influences consumer response to horizontal versus vertical price comparison. Marketing Letters, 26(2): 213–223

  7. Biswas A, Blair EA (1991) Contextual effects of reference prices in retail advertisements. J Mark 55(July):1–12

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Biswas A, Bhowmick S, Guha A, Grewal D (2013) Consumer evaluations of sale prices: role of the subtraction principle. J Mark 77(July):49–66

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Choi P, Coulter KS (2012) It’s not all relative: the effects of mental and physical positioning of comparative prices on absolute versus relative discount assessment. J Retail 88(4):512–527

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Cian L, Krishna A, Schwarz N (2015) Positioning rationality and emotion: rationality is up and emotion is down. J Consum Res 42(4):632–651

    Google Scholar 

  11. Coulter KS, Coulter RA (2005). Size does matter: the effects of magnitude representation congruency on price perceptions and purchase likelihood. J Consum Psychol 15(1): 64–76

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Coulter KS, Norberg PA (2009) The effects of physical distance between regular and sale prices on numerical difference perceptions. J Consum Psychol 19(2): 144–157

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Escalas JE, Bettman JR (2005) Self-construal, reference groups, and brand meaning. J Consum Res 32(3):378–389

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Grewal D, Monroe KB, Krishnan R (1998) The effects of price-comparison advertising on buyers’ perceptions of acquisition value, transaction value, and behavioral intentions. J Mark 62(April):46–59

    Google Scholar 

  15. Grewal D, Ailawadi KL, Gauri D, Hall K, Kopalle P, Robertson JR (2011) Innovations in retail pricing and promotion. J Retail 87(1):S43–S52

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Grewal D, Roggeveen AL, Nordfält J (2017) The future of retailing. J Retail 93(1):1–6

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hayes AF (2015) An index and test of linear moderated mediation. Multivar Behav Res 50(1):1–22

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Hofstede G (1984) Culture’s consequences: international differences in work-related values. Abridged Edition. Sage, Beverly Hills

  19. Hofstede G (1991) Cultures and Organizations: Software of the Mind. McGraw-Hill, London

  20. Jost JT (2017) The marketplace of ideology: ‘Elective affinities’ in political psychology and their implications for consumer behavior. J Consum Psychol 27(4):502–520

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Keefer LA, Landau MJ, Sullivan D, Rothschild ZK (2011) Exploring metaphor’s epistemic function: uncertainty moderates metaphor-consistent priming effects on social perceptions. J Exp Soc Psychol 47(3):657–660

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Krishna A, Briesch R, Lehmann DR, Yuan H (2002) A meta-analysis of the impact of price presentation on perceived savings. J Retail 78(2):101–118

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Labroo AA, Dhar R, Schwarz N (2008) Of frog wines and frowning watches: semantic priming, perceptual fluency, and brand evaluation. J Consum Res 34(6):819–831

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Lalwani AK, Forcum L (2016) Does a dollar get you a dollar’s worth of merchandise? The impact of power distance belief on price-quality judgments. J Consum Res 43(2):317–333

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Lee AY, Aaker JL (2004) Bringing the frame into focus: the influence of regulatory fit on processing fluency and persuasion. J Pers Soc Psychol 86(2):205–218

    Article  Google Scholar 

  26. Maclnnis DJ, Jaworski BJ (1989) Information processing from advertisements: toward an integrative framework. J Mark 53(4):1–23

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Mead JA, Hardesty DM. (2018) Price font disfluency: anchoring effects on future price expectations. J Retail 94(1): 102–112

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Meyers-Levy J (2006) Using the horizontal/vertical distinction to advance insights into consumer psychology. J Consum Psychol 16(4):347–351

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Novemsky N, Dhar R, Schwarz N, Simonson I (2007) Preference fluency in choice. J Mark Res 44(3):347–356

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Ordabayeva N, Fernandes D (2018) Better or different? How political ideology shapes preferences for differentiation in the social hierarchy. J Consum Res in press

  31. Oyserman D (2006) High power, low power, and equality: culture beyond individuals and collectivism. J Consum Psychol 16(4):353–357

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Oyserman D (2009) Identity-based motivation: implications for action-readiness, procedural-readiness, and consumer behavior. J Consum Psychol 19(3):250–260

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. Petty RE, Cacioppo JT (1986) The elaboration likelihood model of persuasion. In: Communication and persuasion. Springer, New York, pp 1–24

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  34. Pratto F, Sidanius J, Stallworth LM, Malle BF (1994) Social dominance orientation: a personality variable predicting social and political attitudes. J Pers Soc Psychol 67(4):741

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Puccinelli NM, Chandrashekaran R, Grewal D, Suri R (2013) Are men seduced by red? The effect of red versus black prices on price perceptions. J Retail 89(2):115–125

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Robinson MD, Zabelina DL, Ode S, Moeller SK (2008) The vertical nature of dominance-submission: Individual differences in vertical attention. J Res Pers 42(4):933–948

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Rucker DD, Galinsky AD, Dubois D (2012) Power and consumer behavior: how power shapes who and what consumers value. J Consum Psychol 22(3):352–368

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Schwarz N (2011) Feelings-as-information theory. Handbook of Theories of Social Psychology, vol 1. pp 289–308

  39. Shankar V, Inman JJ, Mantrala M, Kelley E, Rizley R (2011) Innovations in shopper marketing: current insights and future research issues. J Retail 87(1):S29–S42

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Shavitt S, Lalwani AK, Zhang J, Torelli CJ (2006) The horizontal/vertical distinction in cross-cultural consumer research. J Consum Psychol 16(4):325–342

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Shavitt S, Torelli CJ, Wong J (2009) Identity-based motivation: constraints and opportunities in consumer research. J Consum Psychol 19(3):261–266

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Thomas M, Morwitz VG (2009) The ease-of-computation effect: the interplay of metacognitive experiences and naive theories in judgments of price differences. J Mark Res 46(1):81–91

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Torelli CJ, Özsomer A, Carvalho SW, Keh HT, Maehle N (2012) Brand concepts as representations of human values: do cultural congruity and compatibility between values matter? J Mark 76(4):92–108

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Triandis HC, Gelfand MJ (1998) Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. J Pers Soc Psychol 74(1):118–128

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Valenzuela A, Raghubir P (2015) Are consumers aware of top–bottom but not of left–right inferences? Implications for shelf space positions. J Exp Psychol Appl 21(3):224–241

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Whittlesea BW (1993) Illusions of familiarity. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn 19(6):1235–1253

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Winterich KP, Barone MJ (2011) Warm glow or cold, hard cash? Social identity effects on consumer choice for donation versus discount promotions. J Mark Res 48(5):855–868

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. Barone.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that there is no conflict of interest.

Appendices

Appendix 1. Measures Used in All Studies

1.1 Study 1

Vertical-horizontal orientation (vertical subscale α = .78; horizontal subscale α = .83):

  1. 1.

    I’d rather depend on myself than others.

  2. 2.

    I rely on myself most of the time; I rarely rely on others.

  3. 3.

    I often do “my own thing.”

  4. 4.

    My personal identity, independent of others, is very important to me.

  5. 5.

    It is important that I do my job better than others*.

  6. 6.

    Winning is everything*.

  7. 7.

    Competition is the law of nature*.

  8. 8.

    When another person does better than I do, I get tense and aroused*.

  9. 9.

    If a coworker gets a prize, I would feel proud.

  10. 10.

    The well-being of my coworkers is important to me.

  11. 11.

    To me, pleasure is spending time with others.

  12. 12.

    I feel good when I cooperate with others.

  13. 13.

    Parents and children must stay together as much as possible*.

  14. 14.

    It is my duty to take care of my family, even when I have to sacrifice what I want*.

  15. 15.

    Family members should stick together, no matter what sacrifices are required*.

  16. 16.

    It is important to me that I respect the decisions made by my groups*.

*denotes vertical orientation.

Intentions to purchase (endpoints: 1 = “not at all likely,” 9 = “very likely”)

Assuming you were in market to buy a pizza cutter, how likely would you be to purchase the Pro Pizza cutter at the sale price?

1.2 Study 2

Vertical-horizontal orientation—see study 1 (vertical subscale, α = .76; horizontal subscale, α = .67)

Intentions to purchase—see study 1

Attention to the sales price (endpoints: 1 = “very little,” 9 = “very much”)

As you were reading the information, how much attention did you pay to the sale price of the Pro Pizza cutter?

Covariates:

SES: Please select the social class of the family you grew up in from the following: “1 = Lower class, 2 = Lower middle class, 3 = Middle class, 4 = Upper middle class, 5 = Upper class”

Sex: male, female.

1.3 Study 3

Deal evaluation (r = .66, p < .001)

Assuming you were in the market to buy floss, how likely would you be to purchase the DentyFloss floss at the sale price? (1 = “not at all likely,” 9 = “very likely”)

At the sale price, the DentyFloss floss is 1 = “not a good deal,” 9 = “a great deal”

Processing motivation (endpoints: 1 = “not at all,” 9 = “a lot”)

Please indicate how you felt while reviewing the information about the DentyFloss floss: “Motivated,” “Involved” (r = .56, p < .001)

Appendix 2. Sample Stimuli Used in Studies

Studies 1 and 2—High Discount Condition

figure a

Study 3—High Discount Condition

figure b

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Barone, M.J., Li, X., Winterich, K.P. et al. Social-Spatial Effects in Pricing: When and How Vertical Orientations Shape Processing of Price Comparisons. Cust. Need. and Solut. 5, 137–145 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40547-018-0090-6

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40547-018-0090-6

Keywords

Navigation