Skip to main content
Log in

Efficacy of Neurofeedback on the Increase of Mindfulness-Related Capacities in Healthy Individuals: a Controlled Trial

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Mindfulness Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Electroencephalogram (EEG) studies of mindfulness have shown it can lead to increases in alpha power, which are similar to those obtained by alpha-based neurofeedback (NF) interventions. It has been hypothesized there may be relationships between mindfulness and NF in terms of the neural pathways through which they induce salutary outcomes. The aim of the study was to evaluate possible changes in mindfulness and cognitive functioning following an alpha-based NF intervention, and the role of alpha power as a mediator of improvements. A controlled, non-randomized, trial with 50 healthy participants was conducted with two experimental conditions: a six-session NF intervention and a waiting-list control group. Both groups were administered mindfulness questionnaires (Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS), Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ)) and cognitive measures (Paced Auditory Serial Addition Task (PASAT)), at pre- and post-test. The NF intervention focused on the up-regulation of upper alpha power. Differences among groups were estimated using ANCOVAs, and mediation assessment through path analyses. Compared to controls, the NF group showed enhanced task-related upper alpha power (effect size (ES) = 1.16, p < 0.001), mindfulness outcomes (MAAS: ES = 0.94, p = 0.004; FFMQ: ES = 1.38, p < 0.001), and a trend of cognitive functioning (PASAT time: ES = 0.59, p = 0.062). Upper alpha power had a mediating effect for cognitive functioning (PASAT errors: indirect effect = 0.81, 95% CI = 0.21–1.85), but not for mindfulness. These results demonstrate the effectiveness of NF for increasing mindfulness in healthy individuals with no previous experience in mindfulness or neurofeedback training, suggesting that NF may be an acceptable method of augmenting mindfulness-related capacities in the general population.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Ahani, A., Wahbeh, H., Nezamfar, H., Miller, M., Erdogmus, D., & Oken, B. (2014). Quantitative change of EEG and respiration signals during mindfulness meditation. Journal of Neuroengineering and Rehabilitation, 11, 87. doi:10.1186/1743-0003-11-87.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Amihai, I., & Kozhevnikov, M. (2014). Arousal vs. relaxation: a comparison of the neurophysiological and cognitive correlates of Vajrayana and Theravada meditative practices. PloS One, 9(7). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102990.

  • Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., & Allen, K. B. (2004). Assessment of mindfulness by self-report: the Kentucky inventory of mindfulness skills. Assessment, 11, 191–206. doi:10.1177/1073191104268029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baer R. A., Smith G. T., Hopkins J., Krietemeyer J., Toney L. (2006). Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment, 13, 27–45. doi: 10.1177/1073191105283504.

  • Brandmeyer, T., & Delorme, A. (2013). Meditation and neurofeedback. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 3–5. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergomi, C., Tschacher, W., & Kupper, Z. (2013). Measuring mindfulness: first steps towards the development of a comprehensive mindfulness scale. Mindfulness, 4(1), 18–32. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0102-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, K. W., & Ryan, R. M. (2003). The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(4), 822–848. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahn, B. R., & Polich, J. (2006). Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychological Bulletin, 132(2), 180–211. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.2.180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cahn, B. R., Delorme, A., & Polich, J. (2010). Occipital gamma activation during Vipassana meditation. Cognitive Process, 11(1), 39–56. doi:10.1007/s10339-009-0352-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cebolla, A., García-Palacios, A., Soler, J., Guillen, V., Baños, R., & Botella, C. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Spanish validation of the Five Facets of Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ). The European Journal of Psychiatry, 26(2), 118–126. doi:10.4321/S0213-61632012000200005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesa, A., & Serretti, A. (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychological Medicine, 40(08), 1239–1252. doi:10.1017/S0033291709991747.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesa, A., Calati, R., & Serretti, A. (2011). Does mindfulness training improve cognitive abilities? A systematic review of neuropsychological findings. Clinical Psychology Review, 31(3), 449–464. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.11.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chow, T., Javan, T., Ros, T., & Frewen, P. (2017). EEG dynamics of mindfulness meditation versus alpha neurofeedback: a sham-controlled study. Mindfulness, 8(3), 572–584. doi:10.1007/s12671-016-0631-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2. Auflage). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Congedo, M. (2002). Eureka! (version3.0) [ComputerSoftware]. Knoxville,TN: Nova Tech EEG Inc. Available online at: www.NovaTechEEG.

  • Dunn, B. R., Hartigan, J. A., & Mikulas, W. L. (1999). Concentration and mindfulness meditations: unique forms of consciousness? Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 24(3), 147–165. doi:10.1023/A:1023498629385.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Escolano, C., Aguilar, M., & Minguez, J. (2011). EEG-based upper alpha neurofeedback training improves working memory performance. In 2011 Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society (pp. 2327–2330). doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2011.6090651.

  • Escolano, C., Navarro-Gil, M., Garcia-Campayo, J., Congedo, M., De Ridder, D., & Minguez, J. (2014). A controlled study on the cognitive effect of alpha neurofeedback training in patients with major depressive disorder. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 8, 296. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00296.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Freunberger, R., Werkle-Bergner, M., Griesmayr, B., Lindenberger, U., & Klimesch, W. (2011). Brain oscillatory correlates of working memory constraints. Brain Research, 1375, 93–102. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2010.12.048.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grossman, P., & van Dam, N. T. (2011). Mindfulness, by any other name: trials and tribulations of sati in western psychology and science. Contemporary Buddhism, 12, 219–239. doi:10.1080/14639947.2011.564841.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gronwall, D. M. A. (1977). Paced auditory serial-addition task: a measure of recovery from concussion. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 44(2), 367–373. doi:10.2466/pms.1977.44.2.367.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gruzelier, J. H. (2014). EEG-neurofeedback for optimising performance. I: a review of cognitive and affective outcome in healthy participants. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 44, 124–141. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.09.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hanslmayr, S., Sauseng, P., Doppelmayr, M., Schabus, M., & Klimesch, W. (2005). Increasing individual upper alpha power by neurofeedback improves cognitive performance in human subjects. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 30(1), 1–10. doi:10.1007/s10484-005-2169-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hinterberger, T., Schmidt, S., Kamei, T., & Walach, H. (2014). Decreased electrophysiological activity represents the conscious state of emptiness in meditation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 99. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00099.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Holzel, B. K., Lazar, S. W., Gard, T., Schuman-Olivier, Z., Vago, D. R., & Ott, U. (2011). How does mindfulness meditation work? Proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(6), 537–559. doi:10.1177/1745691611419671.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, F., Hu, Y., Ma, Z., & Rosenberger, W. F. (2014). Adaptive randomization for balancing over covariates. WIREs Computational Statistics, 6, 288–303. doi:10.1002/wics.1309.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyvarinen, A. (1999). Fast and robust fixed-point algorithms for independent component analysis. IEEE Transactions on Neural Networks, 10(3), 626–634. doi:10.1109/72.761722.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kabat-Zinn, J. (2003). Mindfulness-based interventions in context: past, present, and future. Clinical Psychology: Science and Practice, 10(2), 144–156. doi:10.1093/clipsy.bpg016.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W. (1999). EEG alpha and theta oscillations reflect cognitive and memory performance: a review and analysis. Brain Research Reviews, 29(2–3), 169–195. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(98)00056-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: the inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 63–88. doi:10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jensen, O., & Mazaheri, A. (2010). Shaping functional architecture by oscillatory alpha activity: gating by inhibition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 4, 186. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00186.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lagopoulos, J., Xu, J., Rasmussen, I., Vik, A., Malhi, G. S., Eliassen, C. F., Arntsen, I. E., Saether, J. G., Hollup, S., Holen, A., Davanger, S., & Ellingsen, Ø. (2009). Increased theta and alpha EEG activity during nondirective meditation. The Journal of Alternative and Complementary Medicine, 15(11), 1187–1192. doi:10.1089/acm.2009.0113.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, T. W., Girolami, M., & Sejnowski, T. J. (1999). Independent component analysis using an extended infomax algorithm for mixed subgaussian and supergaussian sources. Neural Computation, 11(2), 417–441. doi:10.1162/089976699300016719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lehmann, D., Faber, P. L., Tei, S., Pascual-Marqui, R. D., Milz, P., & Kochi, K. (2012). Reduced functional connectivity between cortical sources in five meditation traditions detected with lagged coherence using EEG tomography. NeuroImage, 60(2), 1574–1586. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.042.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, K., Chan, P., Grabovac, A., Wilkins-Ho, M., & Perri, M. (2013). Changes in mindfulness following repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for mood disorders. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 58(12), 687–691. doi:10.1177/070674371305801206.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lomas, T., Ivtzan, I., & Fu, C. H. (2015). A systematic review of the neurophysiology of mindfulness on EEG oscillations. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 57, 401–410. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.09.018.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKillop, J., & Anderson, E. J. (2007). Further psychometric validation of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS). Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 29(4), 289–293. doi:10.1007/s10862-007-9045-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martín Asuero, A., Rodríguez Blanco, T., Pujol-Ribera, E., Berenguera, A., Moix, Q., & J. (2013). Effectiveness of a mindfulness program in primary care professionals. Gaceta Sanitaria., 27(6), 521–528. doi:10.1016/j.gaceta.2013.04.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milz, P., Faber, P. L., Lehmann, D., Kochi, K., & Pascual-Marqui, R. D. (2014). sLORETA intracortical lagged coherence during breath counting in meditation-naïve participants. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience., 8(303). doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00303.

  • Montero-Marin, J., Puebla-Guedea, M., Herrera-Mercadal, P., Cebolla, A., Soler, J., Demarzo, M., Vazquez, C., Rodríguez-Bornaetxea, F., & García-Campayo, J. (2016). Psychological effects of a 1-month meditation retreat on experienced meditators: the role of non-attachment. Frontiers in Psychology, 12(7), 1935. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01935.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nan, W., Rodrigues, J. P., Ma, J., Qu, X., Wan, F., Mak, P. I., Mak, P. U., Vai, M. I., & Rosa, A. (2012). Individual alpha neurofeedback training effect on short term memory. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 86(1), 83–87. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.07.182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Niedermeyer, E., & da Silva, F. L. (Eds.). (2005). Electroencephalography: basic principles, clinical applications, and related fields. Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.

  • Niv, S. (2013). Clinical efficacy and potential mechanisms of neurofeedback. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(6), 676–686. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.037.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osman, A., Lamis, D. A., Bagge, C. L., Freedenthal, S., & Barnes, S. M. (2016). The mindful attention awareness scale: further examination of dimensionality, reliability, and concurrent validity estimates. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(2), 189–199. doi:10.1080/00223891.2015.1095761.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rau, H. K., & Williams, P. G. (2016). Dispositional mindfulness: a critical review of construct validation research. Personality and Individual Differences, 93, 32–43. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.035.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sauer, S., Walach, H., Schmidt, S., Hinterberger, T., Lynch, S., Büsing, A., & Kohls, N. (2013). Assessment of mindfulness: a review on the state of the art. Mindfulness, 4, 3–17. doi:10.1007/s12671-012-0122-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shonin, E., Van Gordon, W., & Griffiths, M. D. (2015). Does mindfulness work? British Medical Journal, 351, h6919. doi:10.1136/bmj.h6919.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Soler, J., Tejedor, R., Feliu-Soler, A., Pascual, J. C., Cebolla, A., Soriano, J., Alvarez, E., & Perez, V. (2012). Psychometric proprieties of Spanish version of Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (MAAS). Actas Españolas de Psiquiatría, 40(1).

  • Soler, J., Elices, M., Franquesa, A., Barker, S., Friedlander, P., Feilding, A., Pascual, J. C., & Riba, J. (2016). Exploring the therapeutic potential of ayahuasca: acute intake increases mindfulness-related capacities. Psychopharmacology, 233(5), 823–829. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4162-0.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Teasdale, J. D., Segal, Z., & Williams, J. M. G. (1995). How does cognitive therapy prevent depressive relapse and why should attentional control (mindfulness) training help? Behaviour Research and Therapy, 33(1), 25–39. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(94)E0011-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tombaugh, T. N. (2006). A comprehensive review of the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test (PASAT). Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 21(1), 53–76. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2005.07.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tuladhar, A. M., Huurne, N. T., Schoffelen, J. M., Maris, E., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2007). Parieto-occipital sources account for the increase in alpha activity with working memory load. Human Brain Mapping, 28(8), 785–792. doi:10.1002/hbm.20306.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dijk, H., Schoffelen, J. M., Oostenveld, R., & Jensen, O. (2008). Prestimulus oscillatory activity in the alpha band predicts visual discrimination ability. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(8), 1816–1823. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1853-07.2008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Gordon, W., Shonin, E., Griffiths, M. D., & Singh, N. N. (2015). There is only one mindfulness: why science and Buddhism need to work together. Mindfulness, 6(1), 49–56. doi:10.1007/s12671-014-0379-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Lutterveld, R., Houlihan, S. D., Pal, P., Sacchet, M. D., McFarlane-Blake, C., Patel, P. R., Sullivan, J. S., Ossadtchi, A., Druker, S., Bauer, C., & Brewer, J. A. (2016). Source-space EEG neurofeedback links subjective experience with brain activity during effortless awareness meditation. NeuroImage, 151, 117–127. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.047.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Yu, X., Fumoto, M., Nakatani, Y., Sekiyama, T., Kikuchi, H., Seki, Y., Sato-Suzuki, I., & Arita, H. (2011). Activation of the anterior prefrontal cortex and serotonergic system is associated with improvements in mood and EEG changes induced by Zen meditation practice in novices. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 80(2), 103–111. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.02.004.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zoefel, B., Huster, R. J., & Herrmann, C. S. (2011). Neurofeedback training of the upper alpha frequency band in EEG improves cognitive performance. NeuroImage, 54(2), 1427–1431. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.08.078.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We thank Red de Investigación en Actividades de Prevención y Promoción de la Salud (Research Network on Preventative Activities and Health Promotion) (REDIAPP-RD12/0005/0006) and Red de Excelencia PROMOSAM (PSI2014-56303-REDT) for their support in the development of this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MN: designed and executed the study and assisted with the data analyses. CE: analyzed the data and collaborated with the design and writing of the study. JMM: analyzed the data and collaborated in the writing of the study. JMZ: collaborated with the design and writing of the study. ES: revised the initial draft and collaborated in the writing of the study. JG: designed the study and collaborated with the writing of the study.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Carlos Escolano Marco.

Ethics declarations

The study was approved by the Aragon Ethical Committee (June 26, 2013; PI13/00077).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Electronic Supplementary Material

ESM 1

(DOCX 83 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Navarro Gil, M., Escolano Marco, C., Montero-Marín, J. et al. Efficacy of Neurofeedback on the Increase of Mindfulness-Related Capacities in Healthy Individuals: a Controlled Trial. Mindfulness 9, 303–311 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0775-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-017-0775-1

Keywords

Navigation