Skip to main content
  • Original Paper
  • Published:

Selective attenuation of electrophysiological activity of the dentate gyrus in a social defeat mouse model

Abstract

Current research on stress pathology has revealed a set of molecular and cellular mechanisms through which psychosocial stress impairs brain function. However, there are few studies that have examined how chronic stress exposure alters neuronal activity patterns at a network level. Here, we recorded ensemble neuronal activity patterns of the cortico-hippocampal network from urethane-anesthetized mice that were subjected to repeated social defeat stress. In socially defeated mice, the magnitudes of local field potential signals, including theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma oscillations, were significantly reduced in the dentate gyrus, whereas they remained unchanged in the hippocampus and somatosensory cortex. In accordance with the vast majority of histological and biochemical studies, our evidence from electrophysiological investigations highlights the dentate gyrus as a key brain area that is primarily susceptible to stress-induced dysfunction.

Introduction

Animals show long-lasting deleterious outcomes in the central nervous system in response to a range of challenging environments and stressful experiences. The dentate gyrus (DG) and hippocampus are vulnerable to repeated stress responses and are considered critical to stress-induced brain dysfunctions. Mice exposed to chronic stress show dendritic atrophy of hippocampal pyramidal cells, including shrinkage of dendritic branches and loss of dendritic spines [1, 2], concomitant with a disruption of functional synaptic plasticity [3, 4]. The integration of such detrimental effects at the cellular level is considered to impair hippocampus-dependent memory [5–7]. In the DG, a stressful experience suppresses the proliferation of newborn granule neurons [8], which is suggested to be a primary cause of psychiatric symptoms, such as enhanced anxiety and depressive-like behavior [9, 10].

Although accumulating evidence implicates the cellular mechanisms underlying stress-induced pathogenesis [11], little is known about how the microscopic phenomena are integrated into the collective activity patterns of neuronal populations. In the mammalian forebrain, neuronal populations produce a variety of organized oscillations within a frequency band of up to hundreds of Hz, depending on the behavioral state [12], which represents the sum of individual neuronal spikes and neurotransmission. Investigation of these oscillatory patterns could form a bridge between cellular and behavioral evidence and uncover further mechanisms underlying mental states and behavioral phenotypes.

Motivated by this research background, we examined how the mouse dentate-hippocampal network undergoes degenerative changes in the collective activity patterns following chronic stress. In the present study, we adopted a social defeat paradigm that has been shown to induce pathophysiological consequences, including inhibition of neurogenesis [13, 14] and enduring behavioral outcomes such as increased anxiety, social aversion, and depression-like syndromes [15–17]. Power spectral analyses on individual local field potential (LFP) data obtained from multiple cortical regions revealed that the DG network specifically shows prominent changes in the power of LFP oscillations.

Materials and methods

Animals

All experiments were performed with the approval of the animal experiment ethics committee at the University of Tokyo (approval number: P24-70) and according to the NIH guidelines for the care and use of animals. A total of 17 male C57BL/6 mice (6–9 weeks old, 21.5–25.2 g) and 6 male CD-1 mice (10 weeks old, 36.9–49.8 g) were used as intruder and resident groups, respectively. Resident male mice were heavier than intruder male mice. A total of 15 male C57BL/6 mice (6–9 weeks old, 20.3–25.3 g) were used as naïve mice. The animals were maintained on a 12-h light/12-h dark schedule with lights off at 7:00 a.m. All animals were purchased from SLC (Shizuoka, Japan).

Social defeat

A chronic social defeat stress model was produced as previously described [15], except that an intruder mouse was exposed daily to social defeat by two different resident mice. At least 1 week before beginning the experiment, all resident mice were singly housed on one side of a home cage (termed resident area; 42.5 cm × 26.6 cm × 15.5 cm), which was divided into two exact halves by a transparent Plexiglas partition (0.5 cm × 41.8 cm × 16.5 cm) with perforated holes, each with a diameter of 5 mm. The bedding in the resident area was left unchanged during this preoperative period. An experimental intruder mouse was exposed to social defeat stress by introducing it into the resident area of a resident mouse for a full 10-min interaction. After the first 10 min of physical contact, the intruder mouse was placed in the cage of another unfamiliar resident mouse and exposed to a second social defeat for a full 10-min interaction. After the second 10 min of physical contact, the intruder mouse was transferred across the partition and placed into the opposite compartment of the second resident home cage for the following 24 h, which allowed the intruder mouse to receive sensory contact from the resident mouse without physical contact. Over the following 12 consecutive days, the intruder mouse was exposed to a new resident mouse so that they did not habituate to interactions with the same residents. Intruder mice were defined as defeated mice based on the following criteria: (1) they were attacked by at least one resident mouse, and (2) the intruder mouse showed signs of subordination, such as upright submissive postures and freezing.

Electrophysiological recording

Electrophysiological recordings were performed 24 h after the final social defeat stress. The mice were anesthetized with urethane (1.0 g/kg, intraperitoneal) and were fixed with a metal head-holding plate. A craniotomy (2.0 × 2.0 mm2) centered at 1.4 mm posterior and 1.4 mm lateral to the bregma was made using a high-speed drill, and the dura was surgically removed. Two stainless screws were implanted in the bone above the cerebellum and served as ground and reference electrodes during the recordings. A microdrive that held the electrode assembly and consisted of 6–8 electrode wires was designed and then created by a 3-D printer. Out of these 6–8 electrodes, electrodes with a length of 1–3 mm stuck out from the microdrive tip. The microdrive included one or two 1-mm electrodes for the somatosensory cortex, two or three 2-mm electrodes for the hippocampus, and two or three 3-mm electrodes for the DG. The assembly was inserted into the brain to a depth of up to 3 mm at a speed of 10 μm/s. In all animals tested, the electrodes were stabilized at their final positions for 30 min and then recording started. In our previous study, we confirmed that this period was sufficient to obtain stable recording and the power of local field potentials remained stable for up to 80 min [18]. We thus chose the first 10 min of the recording period for the following analyses. Periods including massive electrical noise due to touching the recording device or moving electrical cables attached to the device for noise cancellation, were discarded from the analyses. The electrodes were constructed from 17 μm of polyimide-coated platinum–iridium (90/10 %) wire (California Fine Wire, USA), and the electrode tips were plated with platinum to lower the electrode impedances to 150–300 kΩ measured at 1 kHz. To aid the reconstruction of the electrode tracks, the electrodes were coated with DiI fluorophores by dipping the electrode tip into DiI solution (80 mg/ml) dissolved in 1:1 acetone/ethanol for 60 s before recording. Electrophysiological data were sampled and digitized at 2 kHz using a Cereplex direct recording system (Blackrock). Each recording started after stable signals were identified. After finishing a recording, the electrodes stained with DiI were carefully removed from the brain.

Histology

The electrode tracks stained with DiI were identified in histological tissue postmortem. Mice were perfused intracardially with cold 4 % paraformaldehyde in 25 mM phosphate-buffered saline and then decapitated. The brains were coronally sectioned at a thickness of 150 μm and cover slipped with Permount. Recordings from electrodes were included in the data analysis if the electrode’s deepest position was located up to 250 μm from the nearest cell layer.

Data analysis

In all analyses, datasets obtained from multiple recording sites in a brain region in the same animal were averaged so that the number of samples corresponds with the number of animals. The power spectrum of a 10-min LFP signal was calculated by fast Fourier transformation in Matlab (Mathworks). For performing the Fourier transformation, original unfiltered LFP signals were used. For the detection of sharp wave ripple events, LFP signals were band-pass filtered at 150–250 Hz, and the root mean-square power was calculated in the band with a bin size of 20 ms. The threshold for ripple detection was set to 3 standard deviations (SDs) above the mean.

All values are reported as the mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Student’s t-test was performed to identify significant differences between naïve and defeated groups.

Results

Dentate network activity is susceptible to social defeat

We examined how repeated exposure to such social stress corrupts the organized activity patterns of neuronal networks by recording LFP activity from mice chronically exposed to a 12-day social defeat paradigm. Here, each C57BL/6 J intruder mouse underwent social defeat daily that consisted of exposure to two larger and aggressive resident mice for 10 min each, and over 12 consecutive days. After the second 10-min social defeat session, the intruder mouse was singly housed in the opposite side of the home cage of the resident mouse for an imposing sensory contact. The same experimental protocol has been shown to induce an impairment of neurogenesis in the DG [14]. Naïve littermates were not subjected to social defeat and were group housed with their cage mates for at least 7 days after arrival; these mice were used for comparison with defeated animals.

The electrophysiological activity patterns of cortical networks were examined in the defeated mice. On the next day after the 12-day social defeat, mice were anesthetized with urethane and 6–8 electrodes were simultaneously inserted into the deep layers of the somatosensory cortex, the dorsal hippocampus, and the dorsal DG. After stabilizing the electrodes at their recording sites for 30 min, LFP signals were continuously recorded for 20 min. In total, recordings were obtained from 16 naïve mice and 12 defeated mice. In the cortical regions, LFP patterns were classified typically into sub-frequency bands, including theta (4–10 Hz), slow gamma (20–45 Hz), and fast gamma (65–140 Hz) bands (Fig. 1). We analyzed the power of the LFP signals at the individual frequency bands, which served as a measure of the level of neuronal network activity in each brain area. In the DG of the defeated mice, the LFP power at theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma frequency bands were significantly smaller than those in naïve mice (Fig. 2; 8 and 11 recording sites from n = 5 naïve and 5 defeated animals, respectively; theta, t 8 = 2.37, P = 0.045; slow gamma, t 8 = 3.34, P = 0.010; fast gamma, t 8 = 3.36, P = 0.0099). All these DG recordings were from the granule cell layer. This result suggests that the DG network of defeated mice has a decreased ability to create organized oscillatory activity patterns. By contrast, no significant differences in LFP power were observed at all frequency bands in the hippocampal CA1 area between naïve and defeated mice (Fig. 3a, b; 9 and 6 recording sites from n = 5 naïve and 3 defeated animals, respectively; theta, t 6 = 0.99, P = 0.36; slow gamma, t 6 = 1.13, P = 0.30; fast gamma, t 6 = 1.03, P = 0.34). Similar results were obtained from the CA3 area (Fig. 3d, e; 6 and 4 recording sites from n = 4 naïve and 4 defeated animals, respectively; theta, t 6 = 0.49, P = 0.64; slow gamma, t 6 = 0.19, P = 0.86; fast gamma, t 6 = 0.12, P = 0.91). In addition, the frequency of occurrence of hippocampal ripple events, which represent synchronous firing of hippocampal pyramidal cells [19], were not different between naïve and defeated mice (Fig. 3c, f; CA1: t 6 = 0.22, P = 0.84; CA3: t 6 = 0.07, P = 0.94). Similarly, we failed to observe significant differences in the power of oscillatory activities in the somatosensory cortex between the two mouse groups (Fig. 4; 9 and 5 recording sites from n = 5 naïve and 3 defeated animals, respectively; theta, t 6 = 0.73, P = 0.49; slow gamma, t 6 = 1.08, P = 0.32; fast gamma, t 6 = 0.58, P = 0.59). These results suggest that the oscillatory activity of hippocampal and somatosensory cortical networks is more resistant to social defeat than that of the dentate network.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Analysis of an LFP trace. a Schematic illustration of a multisite recording from a urethane-anesthetized mouse. A microdrive that held the electrode assembly, including 6–8 electrode wires, was inserted into the brain. b A representative LFP recording from the DG of a naïve mouse in vivo. Unfiltered and filtered (theta 4–10 Hz; slow γ 25–40 Hz; fast γ 65–140 Hz) LFP traces are shown from top to bottom. Band-pass filtering was performed only for displaying these traces (color figure online)

Fig. 2
figure 2

Social defeat stress decreases neuronal network activity in the DG. a Histological verification of a recording site in a brain section. The yellow arrow indicates the tip of the electrode tract. b Comparison of LFP power in theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma bands between naïve and defeated mice. *P < 0.05, Student’s t-test (color figure online)

Fig. 3
figure 3

Social defeat stress does not affect neuronal network activity in the hippocampus. a Histological verification of a recording site in hippocampal CA1 area. The yellow arrow indicates the tip of the electrode tract. b Comparison of LFP power in theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma bands between naïve and defeated mice. P > 0.05, Student’s t-test. c A representative unfiltered hippocampal LFP trace (top) and its filtered (150–250 Hz) LFP trace (bottom). A ripple event is marked by the red dot. The average number of ripple events per second is shown in the right panel. d Histological verification of a recording site in hippocampal CA3 area. The yellow arrow indicates the tip of the electrode tract. e Comparison of LFP power in theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma bands between naïve and defeated mice. P > 0.05, Student’s t-test. f Comparison of the average number of ripple events per second between naïve and defeated mice. P > 0.05, Student’s t-test (color figure online)

Fig. 4
figure 4

Social defeat stress does not affect neuronal network activity in the somatosensory cortex. a Histological verification of a recording site in a brain section. The yellow arrow indicates the tip of the electrode tract. b Comparison of LFP power in theta, slow gamma, and fast gamma bands between naïve and defeated mice. P > 0.05, Student’s t-test (color figure online)

Discussion

We employed a social defeat model to investigate stress-induced changes in neuronal activity patterns and found that the theta and gamma LFP power was depressed specifically in the DG, whereas the oscillatory power of the hippocampus and somatosensory cortex remained intact. These results suggest that the level of neuronal network activity in the DG is more sensitive to social stress, compared with other cortical regions. This suggestion is consistent with the notion that the DG is an important brain structure vulnerable to stressful experiences, demonstrated by the studies of morphological changes [20, 21], synaptic plasticity [22], cell proliferation [8], and up/down regulation of functional molecules [23–26] in granule cells of the DG.

Adult-born granule neurons are functionally integrated into the dentate circuitry as they become mature. The recently generated granule cells show enhanced excitability and synaptic plasticity [27, 28] that has been proposed to play crucial roles in memory functions, such as spatial memory and pattern separation [29–33], and amelioration of anxiety and depression-like behaviors [9, 10]. Repeated exposures to social defeat stress in rodents cause inhibition of dentate neurogenesis [14] and anxiety, social aversion, and depression-like syndromes [15, 16]. In this study, we employed the same experimental model and demonstrated that the defeat stress led to a prominent decrease in the LFP power of typical network oscillations, such as theta and gamma waves, in the DG. Combined with the histological evidence that newborn granule cells account for a fraction (~5 %) of the total granule cells in the adult DG [32], our result suggests that a greater participation of such a small fraction of newborn granule cells in the dentate network is crucial for sustaining the overall DG network activity level. Assuming that network oscillations represent organized neuronal activity, our results suggest that synchronous firing of dentate granule cells is suppressed in defeated animals. Based on the evidence that lesioning the dentate gyrus results in decreased spatial working memory [34], reduced ability of pattern separation [35], and abnormal anxiety-based behaviors [36], stress-induced attenuation of DG activity may particularly lead to such behavioral deficits.

In hippocampal pyramidal cells, repeated exposure to stress triggers the shrinkage of dendritic arbors, the loss of dendritic spines and the attenuation of synaptic plasticity through the elevation of glucocorticoid levels, which might presumably exert a profound impact on learning and memory functions. However, our LFP analysis failed to detect an effect of chronic social stress exposure on hippocampal network activity, suggesting that intact hippocampal oscillatory activity can emerge under urethane anesthesia even in animals with stress-induced impairment in hippocampus-dependent memory. We thus suggest that the behavioral impairments found in previous studies [15–17] might be mainly due to the attenuation of activity levels in the DG circuit rather than hippocampal and somatosensory cortical circuits.

Finally, we note that social stress is also known to cause neurophysiological changes in other brain regions. Further studies are therefore required to quantitatively determine to what extent the degenerative activity of the dentate-hippocampal network is responsible for stress-induced brain dysfunctions.

References

  1. McEwen BS (2015) Stress-induced remodeling of hippocampal CA3 pyramidal neurons. Brain Res 1645:50–54

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Christian KM, Miracle AD, Wellman CL, Nakazawa K (2011) Chronic stress-induced hippocampal dendritic retraction requires CA3 NMDA receptors. Neuroscience 174:26–36

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Pavlides C, Nivon LG, McEwen BS (2002) Effects of chronic stress on hippocampal long-term potentiation. Hippocampus 12:245–257

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Shors TJ, Seib TB, Levine S, Thompson RF (1989) Inescapable versus escapable shock modulates long-term potentiation in the rat hippocampus. Science 244:224–226

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Luine V, Villegas M, Martinez C, McEwen BS (1994) Repeated stress causes reversible impairments of spatial memory performance. Brain Res 639:167–170

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Schmidt MV, Trumbach D, Weber P, Wagner K, Scharf SH, Liebl C et al (2010) Individual stress vulnerability is predicted by short-term memory and AMPA receptor subunit ratio in the hippocampus. J Neurosci 30:16949–16958

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Pawlak R, Rao BS, Melchor JP, Chattarji S, McEwen B, Strickland S (2005) Tissue plasminogen activator and plasminogen mediate stress-induced decline of neuronal and cognitive functions in the mouse hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 102:18201–18206

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Levone BR, Cryan JF, O’Leary OF (2015) Role of adult hippocampal neurogenesis in stress resilience. Neurobiol Stress 1:147–155

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Miller BR, Hen R (2015) The current state of the neurogenic theory of depression and anxiety. Curr Opin Neurobiol 30:51–58

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Zhao C, Deng W, Gage FH (2008) Mechanisms and functional implications of adult neurogenesis. Cell 132:645–660

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. McEwen BS, Bowles NP, Gray JD, Hill MN, Hunter RG, Karatsoreos IN et al (2015) Mechanisms of stress in the brain. Nat Neurosci 18:1353–1363

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Buzsaki G (2006) Rhythms of the brain. Oxford University Press, New York

    Book  Google Scholar 

  13. Simon M, Czeh B, Fuchs E (2005) Age-dependent susceptibility of adult hippocampal cell proliferation to chronic psychosocial stress. Brain Res 1049:244–248

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Schloesser RJ, Lehmann M, Martinowich K, Manji HK, Herkenham M (2010) Environmental enrichment requires adult neurogenesis to facilitate the recovery from psychosocial stress. Mol Psychiatry 15:1152–1163

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  15. Golden SA, Covington HE III, Berton O, Russo SJ (2011) A standardized protocol for repeated social defeat stress in mice. Nat Protoc 6:1183–1191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Berton O, McClung CA, Dileone RJ, Krishnan V, Renthal W, Russo SJ et al (2006) Essential role of BDNF in the mesolimbic dopamine pathway in social defeat stress. Science 311:864–868

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Rygula R, Abumaria N, Domenici E, Hiemke C, Fuchs E (2006) Effects of fluoxetine on behavioral deficits evoked by chronic social stress in rats. Behav Brain Res 174:188–192

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Nishimura Y, Abe R, Sasaki T, Ikegaya Y (2016) Homeostatic changes in neuronal network oscillations in response to continuous hypoperfusion in the mouse forebrain. Neurosci Res 109:28–34

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Csicsvari J, Hirase H, Mamiya A, Buzsaki G (2000) Ensemble patterns of hippocampal CA3-CA1 neurons during sharp wave-associated population events. Neuron 28:585–594

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sousa N, Lukoyanov NV, Madeira MD, Almeida OF, Paula-Barbosa MM (2000) Reorganization of the morphology of hippocampal neurites and synapses after stress-induced damage correlates with behavioral improvement. Neuroscience 97:253–266

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. van der Beek EM, Wiegant VM, Schouten WG, van Eerdenburg FJ, Loijens LW, van der Plas C et al (2004) Neuronal number, volume, and apoptosis of the left dentate gyrus of chronically stressed pigs correlate negatively with basal saliva cortisol levels. Hippocampus 14:688–700

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Shors TJ, Dryver E (1994) Effect of stress and long-term potentiation (LTP) on subsequent LTP and the theta burst response in the dentate gyrus. Brain Res 666:232–238

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Conrad CD, McEwen BS (2000) Acute stress increases neuropeptide Y mRNA within the arcuate nucleus and hilus of the dentate gyrus. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 79:102–109

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Rasmusson AM, Shi L, Duman R (2002) Downregulation of BDNF mRNA in the hippocampal dentate gyrus after re-exposure to cues previously associated with footshock. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:133–142

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Bilang-Bleuel A, Ulbricht S, Chandramohan Y, De Carli S, Droste SK, Reul JM (2005) Psychological stress increases histone H3 phosphorylation in adult dentate gyrus granule neurons: involvement in a glucocorticoid receptor-dependent behavioural response. Eur J Neurosci 22:1691–1700

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Heine VM, Zareno J, Maslam S, Joels M, Lucassen PJ (2005) Chronic stress in the adult dentate gyrus reduces cell proliferation near the vasculature and VEGF and Flk-1 protein expression. Eur J Neurosci 21:1304–1314

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Schmidt-Hieber C, Jonas P, Bischofberger J (2004) Enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated granule cells of the adult hippocampus. Nature 429:184–187

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Ge S, Yang CH, Hsu KS, Ming GL, Song H (2007) A critical period for enhanced synaptic plasticity in newly generated neurons of the adult brain. Neuron 54:559–566

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. Nakashiba T, Cushman JD, Pelkey KA, Renaudineau S, Buhl DL, McHugh TJ et al (2012) Young dentate granule cells mediate pattern separation, whereas old granule cells facilitate pattern completion. Cell 149:188–201

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Clelland CD, Choi M, Romberg C, Clemenson GD Jr, Fragniere A, Tyers P et al (2009) A functional role for adult hippocampal neurogenesis in spatial pattern separation. Science 325:210–213

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Sahay A, Scobie KN, Hill AS, O’Carroll CM, Kheirbek MA, Burghardt NS et al (2011) Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation. Nature 472:466–470

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Imayoshi I, Sakamoto M, Ohtsuka T, Takao K, Miyakawa T, Yamaguchi M et al (2008) Roles of continuous neurogenesis in the structural and functional integrity of the adult forebrain. Nat Neurosci 11:1153–1161

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Piatti VC, Ewell LA, Leutgeb JK (2013) Neurogenesis in the dentate gyrus: carrying the message or dictating the tone. Front Neurosci 7:50

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Niewoehner B, Single FN, Hvalby O, Jensen V, Meyer zum Alten Borgloh S, Seeburg PH et al (2007) Impaired spatial working memory but spared spatial reference memory following functional loss of NMDA receptors in the dentate gyrus. Eur J Neurosci 25:837–846

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Gilbert PE, Kesner RP, Lee I (2001) Dissociating hippocampal subregions: double dissociation between dentate gyrus and CA1. Hippocampus 11:626–636

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Weeden CS, Roberts JM, Kamm AM, Kesner RP (2015) The role of the ventral dentate gyrus in anxiety-based behaviors. Neurobiol Learn Mem 118:143–149

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by Kaken-hi (15H05569; 15H01417), Yamada Research Grant, Mochida Memorial Foundation for Medical and Pharmaceutical Research, and Astellas Foundation for Research on Metabolic Disorders.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Takuya Sasaki.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in studies involving animals were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institution or practice at which the studies were conducted.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Aoki, Y., Nishimura, Y., Hondrich, T. et al. Selective attenuation of electrophysiological activity of the dentate gyrus in a social defeat mouse model. J Physiol Sci 67, 507–513 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12576-016-0481-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12576-016-0481-0

Keywords