Skip to main content
Log in

Altruistic Behavior among Twins

Willingness to Fight and Self-Sacrifice for Their Closest Relatives

  • Published:
Human Nature Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Correction to this article was published on 22 November 2017

This article has been updated

Abstract

According to kin selection theory, indirect reproductive advantages may induce individuals to care for others with whom they share genes by common descent, and the amount of care, including self-sacrifice, will increase with the proportion of genes shared. Twins represent a natural situation in which this hypothesis can be tested. Twin pairs experience the same early environment because they were born and raised at the same time and in the same family but their genetic relatedness differs depending on zygosity. We compared the degree of willingness to fight and sacrifice for the co-twin among monozygotic (MZ) and dizygotic (DZ) pairs in a sample of 1443 same-sex and opposite-sex twins. We also analyzed the effect of the subject’s gender and that of the co-twin on those altruistic behaviors. Results partly supported the postulated explanation. MZ twins (who share nearly their entire genome) were significantly more likely than DZ twins (who on average share half of their segregating genes) to self-sacrifice for their co-twins, but zygosity did not affect willingness to fight for him/her. The genders of the subject and of the co-twin, not genetic relatedness, were the best predictors of aggressive altruistic intentions.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 22 November 2017

    The original version of this article unfortunately contained a mistake. The presentation of the article title and subtitle was incorrect.

References

  • Archer, J. (2009). Does sexual selection explain human sex differences in aggression? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 249–266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettencourt, B., & Kernahan, C. (1997). A meta-analysis of aggression in the presence of violent cues: effects of gender differences and aversive provocation. Aggressive Behavior, 23, 447–456.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bettencourt, B., & Miller, N. (1996). Gender differences in aggression as a function of provocation: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 119, 422–447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burnstein, E. (2005). Altruism and genetic relatedness. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 528–551). NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Burnstein, E., Crandall, C., & Kitayama, S. (1994). Some neo-Darwinian decision rules for altruism: weighing cues for inclusive fitness as a function of the biological importance of the decision. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 773–789.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buss, D. M., & Shackelford, T. K. (1997). Human aggression in evolutionary psychological perspective. Clinical Psychology Review, 17, 605–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, A. (2013). The evolutionary psychology of women’s aggression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 368(1631), 20130078.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Danby, S., & Thorpe, K. (2006). Compatibility and conflict: negotiation of relationships by dizygotic same-sex twin girls. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 103–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diggle, P., Heagerty, J. P., Liang, K.-Y., & Zeger, S. L. (2002). The analysis of longitudinal data (2nd ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Crowley, M. (1986). Gender and helping behavior: a meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 283–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1986). Gender and aggressive behavior: a meta-analytic review of the social psychological literature. Psychological Bulletin, 100, 309–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Wood, W. (1999). The origins of sex differences in human behavior: evolved dispositions versus social roles. American Psychologist, 54, 408–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitzgerald, C. J., & Ketterer, H. L. (2011). Examining verbal and physical retaliation against kinship insults. Violence and Victims, 26, 580–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortuna, K., Goldner, I., & Knafo, A. (2010). Twin relationships: a comparison across monozygotic twins, dizygotic twins, and nontwin siblings in early childhood. Family Science, 1, 205–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foy, A. K., Vernon, P. A., & Jang, K. (2001). Examining the dimensions of intimacy in twin and peer relationships. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 4, 443–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gesselman, A. N., & Webster, G. D. (2012). Inclusive fitness affects both prosocial and antisocial behavior: target gender and insult domain moderate the link between genetic relatedness and aggression. Evolutionary Psychology, 10, 750–761.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Giles, J. W., & Heyman, G. D. (2005). Young children's beliefs about the relationship between gender and aggressive behavior. Child Development, 76, 107–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). An ambivalent alliance: hostile and benevolent sexism as complementary justifications for gender inequality. American Psychologist, 56, 109–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, A., & Vázquez, A. (2015). The power of “feeling one” with a group: identity fusion and extreme pro-group behaviours. International Journal of Social Psychology, 30, 481–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gómez, A., Brooks, M. L., Buhrmester, M. D., Vázquez, A., Jetten, J., & Swann Jr., W. B. (2011). On the nature of identity fusion: insights into the construct and a new measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100, 918–933.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grafen, A. (2006). Optimization of inclusive fitness. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 238, 541–563.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guerra, V. M., Giner-Sorolla, R., & Vasiljevic, M. (2013). The importance of honor concerns across eight countries. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 16, 298–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackman, J., Munira, S., Jasmin, K., & Hruschka, D. (2017). Revisiting psychological mechanisms in the anthropology of altruism. Human Nature, 28, 76–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7, 1–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katz, M. H. (2011). Multivariable analysis: a practical guide for clinicians and public health researchers. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kenrick, D. T., Ackerman, J. M., & Ledlow, S. (2003). Evolutionary social psychology: Adaptive predispositions and human culture. In J. Delamater (Ed.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 103–122). New York: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korchmaros, J. D., & Kenny, D. A. (2006). An evolutionary and close-relationship model of helping. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 23, 21–43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurland, J. A., & Gaulin, S. J. (2005). Cooperation and conflict among kin. In D. M. Buss (Ed.), The handbook of evolutionary psychology (pp. 447–482). NY: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lickel, B., Miller, N., Stenstrom, D. M., Denson, T. F., & Schmader, T. (2006). Vicarious retribution: the role of collective blame in intergroup aggression. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10, 372–390.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loh, C. Y., & Elliott, J. M. (1998). Cooperation and competition as a function of zygosity in 7-to 9-year-old twins. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19, 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Madsen, E. A., Tunney, R. J., Fieldman, G., Plotkin, H. C., Dunbar, R. I., Richardson, J. M., & McFarland, D. (2007). Kinship and altruism: a cross-cultural experimental study. British Journal of Psychology, 98, 339–359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mark, K. M., Pike, A., Latham, R. M., & Oliver, B. R. (2017). Using twins to better understand sibling relationships. Behavior Genetics, 47, 202–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mikkelson, A. C., Floyd, K., & Pauley, P. M. (2011). Differential solicitude of social support in different types of adult sibling relationships. Journal of Family Communication, 11, 220–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, D. M., Varnell, S. P., & Blitstein, J. L. (2004). Design and analysis of group-randomized trials: a review of recent methodological developments. American Journal of Public Health, 94, 423–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neyer, F. J. (2002a). The dyadic interdependence of attachment security and dependency: a conceptual replication across older twin pairs and younger couples. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 483–503.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neyer, F. J. (2002b). Twin relationships in old age: A developmental perspective. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 19, 155–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neyer, F. J., & Lang, F. R. (2003). Blood is thicker than water: kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 310–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ordoñana, J. R., Pérez-Riquelme, F., González-Javier, F., Carrillo, E., Gómez-Amor, J., & Martínez-Selva, J. M. (2006). An initiative in Spain for the study of women’s health: the Murcia twin registry. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 9, 865–867.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ordoñana, J. R., Rebollo-Mesa, I., Carrillo, E., Colodro-Conde, L., García-Palomo, F. J., González-Javier, F., et al. (2013). The Murcia twin registry: a population-based registry of adult multiples in Spain. Twin Research and Human Genetics, 16, 302–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osiński, J. (2009). Kin altruism, reciprocal altruism and social discounting. Personality and Individual Differences, 47, 374–378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Park, J. H., & Ackerman, J. M. (2011). Passion and compassion: Psychology of kin relations within and beyond the family. In T. K. Shackelford & C. A. Salmon (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary family psychology (pp. 329–344). NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penninkilampi-Kerola, V., Moilanen, I., & Kaprio, J. (2005). Co-twin dependence, social interactions, and academic achievement: a population-based study. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 519–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderheiser, J. M. (2013). Behavioral genetics (6th ed.). NY: Worth Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pulkkinen, L., Vaalamo, I., Hietala, R., Kaprio, J., & Rose, J. (2003). Peer reports of adaptive behavior in twins and singletons: is twinship a risk or an advantage? Twin Research, 6, 106–118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, D. R., & Green, L. R. (1999). Social sanction and threat explanations of gender: effects on direct and indirect aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 25, 425–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rodriguez Mosquera, P. M., Fischer, A., Manstead, A., & Zaalberg, R. (2008). Attack, disapproval, or withdrawal? The role of honour in anger and shame responses to being insulted. Cognition and Emotion, 22, 1471–1498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salmivalli, C., & Kaukiainen, A. (2004). “Female aggression” revisited: variable-and person-centered approaches to studying gender differences in different types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior, 30, 158–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L. (2000). Entwined lives: twins and what they tell us about human behavior. NY: Plume.

    Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L., & Ream, S. L. (1998). Decrease in grief intensity for deceased twin and non-twin relatives: an evolutionary perspective. Personality and Individual Differences, 25, 317–325.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L., Wilson, S. M., Bouchard, T. J., & Gitlin, D. G. (1995). Comparative grief experiences of bereaved twins and other bereaved relatives. Personality and Individual Differences, 18, 511–524.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Segal, N. L., Hershberger, N. L., & Arad, S. (2003). Meeting one's twin: perceived social closeness and familiarity. Evolutionary Psychology, 1, 70–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stewart-Williams, S. (2008). Human beings as evolved nepotists. Exceptions to the rule and effects of cost of help. Human Nature, 19, 414–425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swann Jr., W. B., Gómez, A., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J. F., & Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion: the interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96, 995–1011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swann Jr., W. B., Buhrmester, M. D., Gómez, A., Jetten, J., Bastian, B., Vázquez, A., et al. (2014). What makes a group worth dying for? Identity fusion fosters perception of familial ties, promoting self-sacrifice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106, 912–926.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Vugt, M. (2009). Sex differences in intergroup competition, aggression, and warfare. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1167, 124–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G. D. (2003). Prosocial behavior in families: moderators of resource sharing. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 39, 644–652.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G. D. (2004). Human kin investment as a function of genetic relatedness and lineage. Evolutionary Psychology, 2, 129–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G. D., Bryan, A., Crawford, C. B., McCarthy, L., & Cohen, B. H. (2008). Lineage, sex, and wealth as moderators of kin investment. Human Nature, 19, 189–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, G. D., Cottrell, C. A., Schember, T. O., Crysel, L. C., Croiser, B. S., Gesselman, A. N., & Le, B. M. (2012). Two sides of the same coin? Viewing altruism and aggression through the adaptive lens of kinship. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 6, 575–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Workman, L., & Reader, W. (2008). Evolutionary psychology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yzerbyt, V., Dumont, M., Wigboldus, D., & Gordijn, E. (2003). I feel for us: The impact of categorization and identification on emotions and action tendencies. British Journal of Social Psychology, 42, 533–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeger, S. L., & Liang, K. Y. (1986). Longitudinal data analysis for discrete and continuous outcomes. Biometrics, 42, 121–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank the twins participating in the Murcia Twin registry for their collaboration and commitment. The Murcia Twin Registry is supported by Fundación Séneca, Regional Agency for Science and Technology, Murcia, Spain (08633/PHCS/08, 15302/PHCS/10 & 19479/PI/14) and Ministry of Economy and Competitiveness (MINECO), Spain (PSI2009-11560 & PSI2014-56680-R). UNED group is also supported by MINECO, Spain (PSI2015-67754-P).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Juan R. Ordoñana.

Ethics declarations

Ethical Approval

MTR participants were contacted through postal letter invitation, which contained information regarding the objectives of the registry and study information. Twins were later contacted by telephone. Oral informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study, prior to any data collection taking place. All MTR procedures, including informed consent and data collection for this study, was approved by the Murcia University Ethical Committee. National regulations regarding personal data protection were followed. All procedures performed in this study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Additional information

The original version of this article was revised due to incorrect presentation of the article title and subtitle.

A correction to this article is available online at https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9308-9.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Tornero, E., Sánchez-Romera, J.F., Morosoli, J.J. et al. Altruistic Behavior among Twins. Hum Nat 29, 1–12 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9304-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-017-9304-0

Keywords

Navigation