Skip to main content
Log in

Enhancing orthographic knowledge helps spelling production in eight-year-old Chinese children at risk for dyslexia

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigated the effects of enhancing orthographic knowledge on the spelling of Chinese characters and words in 131 eight-year-old Chinese children at risk for dyslexia. The traditional approach (37 children) emphasizing memory and repeated writing was the control condition. The analytic and synthetic approach (ASA, 33 children) stressed insight into character structure. The integrated analytic and synthetic approach added to ASA self-correction and metacognitive activities (INA, 61 children). The children were first asked to write down as many words as possible associated with pictures of home, school, and community; the correctly written words formed the baseline information. The children were then instructed by their classroom teachers in six especially designed short texts and assessed in eight measurable bujian or radical tasks subserving three constructs: morpheme completion, bujian analysis and synthesis and bujian compounding. Multivariate analyses of variance showed that the children in the INA condition outperformed those in the other conditions in three of the measurable bujian tasks. A confirmatory factor analysis verified the stability of the eight tasks and their clustering into three constructs. From these results, we tentatively propose a “bujian sensitivity hypothesis” as a means of helping young Chinese children at risk for spelling disorders.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, R. C., Li, W., Ku, Y.-M., Shu, H., & Wu, N. (2003). Use of partial information in learning to read Chinese characters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 52–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 334–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bishop, D. V. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2004). Developmental dyslexia and specific language impairment: Same or different? Psychological Bulletin, 130, 858–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, T. A. (2006). Confimratory factor analysis for applied research. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, G. D. A., & Ellis, N. (Eds.). (1994). Handbook of spelling: Theory, process, and intervention. Chichester: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chao, Y. R. (1968). A grammar of spoken Chinese. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Hao, M., Geva, E., Zhu, J., & Shu, H. (2009). The role of compound awareness in Chinese children’s vocabulary acquisition and character reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 615–631.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chliounaki, K., & Bryant, P. (2007). How children learn about morphological spelling rules. Child Development, 78, 1360–1373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Conrad, N. J. (2008). From reading to spelling and spelling to reading: Transfer goes both ways. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 869–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, M. (1970). The dyslexic child (2nd and augmented ed. of Developmental dyslexia). London: Heinemann Medical Books.

  • Cunningham, A. E. (2006). Accounting for children’s orthographic learning while reading text: Do children self-teach? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 95, 56–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., Perry, K. E., Stanovich, K. E., & Share, D. L. (2002). Orthographic learning during reading: Examining the role of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 185–199.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P., & Share, D. L. (2007). Orthographic learning during oral and silent reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 55–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1997). Learning to read and learning to spell are one and the same, almost. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 237–269). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elliott, C. D., Murray, D. J., & Pearson, L. S. (1978). British Ability Scales: Manual 3: Directions for administration and scoring and Manual 4: Tables of abilities and norms. Windsor, Berks: NFER.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., Novy, D. M., & Liberman, D. (1991). How letter-sound instruction mediates progress in first-grade reading and spelling. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 456–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M. (2005). Dyslexia—A molecular disorder of neuronal migration: The 2004 Norman Geschwind Memorial Lecture. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 151–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., LoTurco, J., Ramus, F., Fitch, R. H., & Rosen, G. D. (2006). From genes to behavior in developmental dyslexia. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 1213–1217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, E. H. (1990). Teaching spelling (2nd ed.). Boston: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Chung, K. K. H., Tsang, S.-M., Lee, S.-H., & Cheng, R. W.-Y. (2007). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing for primary school students—second edition [HKT-P(II)]. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific Learning Difficulties Research Team.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Lee, S.-H., Tsang, S.-M., & Luan, V. H. (2004). Cognitive profiling and preliminary subtyping in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Cognition, 91, 43–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W.-O., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2002). The cognitive profile and multiple deficit hypothesis in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 38, 543–553.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Ng, T.-T., & Ng, W.-K. (2003). A “radical” approach to reading development in Chinese: The role of semantic radicals and phonetic radicals. Journal of Literacy Research, 35, 849–878.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Wong, W.-L., & Chan, W.-S. (1999). The use of orthographic analogies in learning to read Chinese. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 393–403.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Høien, T., & Lundberg, I. (2000). Dyslexia: From theory to intervention. Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hsiao, J. H.-W., & Shillock, R. (2006). Analysis of a Chinese phonetic compound database: Implications for orthographic processing. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 35, 405–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C. (1983). Multi-sensory teaching and specific reading retardation. London: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., & Joshi, R. M. (Eds.). (1998). Reading and spelling: Development and disorders. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Monk, A., & Ives, S. (1987). Some experimental studies of multi-sensory teaching: The effects of the manual tracing on children’s pair-associative learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 5, 299–307.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jöreskog, K.G., & Sörbom, D. (1996–2001). LISREL 8: User’s reference guide. Chicago: Scientific Software International.

  • Keuning, J., & Verhoeven, L. (2008). Spelling development through the elementary grades: The Dutch case. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 459–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kwan, T.W. (2003). Chinese character database: With word-formations. Retrieved March 15, 2009 from http://humanum.arts.cuhk.edu.hk/Lexis/lexi-can/.

  • Kwong, T. E., & Varnhagen, C. K. (2005). Strategy development and learning to spell new words: Generalization of a process. Developmental Psychology, 41, 148–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Law, S.-P. (2004). Writing errors of a Cantonese dysgraphic patient and their theoretical implications. Neurocase, 10, 132–140.

    Google Scholar 

  • Law, S.-P., Yeung, O., Wong, W., & Chiu, K. M. Y. (2005). Processing of semantic radicals in writing Chinese characters: Data from a Chinese dysgraphic patient. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 885–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1999). What can we learn from dyslexia in Chinese? In I. Lundberg, F. I. Tønnessen, & I. Austad (Eds.), Dyslexia: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 117–139). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (2009). The role of inflectional morphology in Canadian children’s word reading and spelling. The Elementary School Journal, 109, 343–358.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K., Cheng, P. W., & Lam, C. C. (2000). Exploring reading spelling connection as locus of dyslexia in Chinese. Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 239–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K., & Ho, M. K. (2008). The role of lexical knowledge and related linguistic components in typical and poor language comprehenders of Chinese. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 559–586.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R. (1995). Toward a definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 3–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, G. R., Shaywitz, S. E., & Shaywitz, B. A. (2003). A definition of dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Grayson, D. (2005). Goodness of fit in structural equation models. In A. Maydeu-Olivaries & J. J. McArdle (Eds.), Contemporary psychometrics: A Festschrift for Roderick P. McDonald (pp. 225–340). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R. K., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. C. (2000). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., & Bryant, P. (2009). Children’s reading and spelling: Beyond the first steps. Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K. (2006). Genes, environment, and dyslexia: The 2005 Norman Geschwind Memorial Lecture. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 205–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing, and speech problems in children. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. L. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: A linguistic and cognitive approach. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Packard, J. L., Chen, X., Li, W., Wu, X., Gaffney, J. S., Li, H., et al. (2006). Explicit instruction in orthographic structure and word morphology helps Chinese children learn to write characters. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 457–487.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2009). Relations among speech, language, and reading disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 283–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1997). The psycholinguistics of spelling and reading. In C. A. Perfetti, L. Rieben, & M. Fayol (Eds.), Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages (pp. 21–38). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2006). Reading Chinese characters: Orthography, phonology, meaning, and the lexical constituency model. In P. Li, L. H. Tan, E. Bates, & O. J. L. Tzeng (Eds.), The handbook of East Asian psycholinguistics (pp. 225–236). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Liu, Y., & Tan, L. H. (2005). The lexical constituency model: Some implications of research on Chinese for general theories of reading. Psychological Review, 112, 43–59.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Rieben, L., & Fayol, M. (Eds.). (1997). Learning to spell: Research, theory, and practice across languages. Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pollo, T. C., Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2008). Three perspectives on spelling development. In E. L. Grigorenko & A. J. Naples (Eds.), Single-word reading: Behavioral and biological perspectives (pp. 175–189). New York: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Standard progressive matrices: Including the parallel and plus versions. Oxford: Oxford Psychological Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rittle-Johnson, B., & Siegler, R. S. (1999). Learning to spell: Variability, choice, and change in children’s strategy use. Child Development, 70, 332–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shahar-Yames, D., & Share, D. L. (2008). Spelling as a self-teaching mechanism in orthographic learning. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 22–39.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 95–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2004). Phonological recoding at a glance: On the time course and developmental onset of self-teaching. Journal of Experimental Child Psychologh, 87, 267–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sharp, A. C., Sinatra, G. M., & Reynolds, R. E. (2008). The development of children’s orthographic knowledge: A microgenetic perspective. Reading Research Quarterly, 43, 206–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. (2003). Overcoming dyslexia: A new and complete science-based program for reading problems at any level. New York: Alfrd A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shen, H. H., & Bear, D. R. (2000). Development of orthographic skills in Chinese children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 13, 197–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1997). Role of radical awareness in the character and word acquisition of Chinese children. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 78–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (1999). Learning to read Chinese: The development of metalinguistic awareness. In J. Wang, A. B. Inhoff, & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading Chinese script: A cognitive analysis (pp. 1–18). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., Chen, X., Anderson, R. C., Wu, N., & Xuan, Y. (2003). Properties of school Chinese: Implications for learning to read. Child Development, 74, 27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shu, H., McBride-Chang, C., Wu, S., & Liu, H. (2006). Understanding Chinese developmental dyslexia: Morphological awareness as a core cognitive construct. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 122–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Hayiou-Thomas, M. E. (2006). The dyslexia spectrum: Continuities between reading, speech, and language impairment. Topics in Language Disorders, 26, 108–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Hulme, C. (2005). Learning to read with a language impairment. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 397–412). Oxford: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Steffler, D. J., Varnhagen, C. K., Friesen, C. K., & Treiman, R. (1998). There’s more to children’s spelling than the errors they make: Strategic and automatic processes for one-syllable words. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 492–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(24), 8781–8785 (June 14).

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Templeton, S. (2003). Spelling. In J. Flood, D. Lapp, J. R. Squire, & J. M. Jensen (Eds.), Handbook of research on teaching the English language arts (2nd ed., pp. 738–751). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ting, F.-P. (1927/1970). Shuōwén Jiézi Kulin. Taipei: Commercial Press.

  • Tong, X., McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., & Wong, A. M.-Y. (2009). Morphological awareness, orthographic knowledge, and spelling errors: Keys to understanding early Chinese literacy acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 426–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1993). Beginning to spell: A study of first-grade children. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2005). Writing systems and spelling development. In M. J. Snowling & C. Hulme (Eds.), The science of reading: A handbook (pp. 120–134). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S. K. (Ed.). (2001). Gaoxiao hanzi jiao yu xue [Effective teaching and learning of Chinese characters]. Hong Kong: Greenfield Education Publishing (in Chinese).

    Google Scholar 

  • Tse, S.K., Cheung, W.Y.C., Loh, K.Y., & Lui, W.L. (2008). [Chinese language curriculum, teaching materials, and teaching methods: Help for children with special needs]. Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press (in Chinese).

  • Tse, S. K., Marton, F., Ki, W. W., & Loh, E. K. Y. (2007). An integrative perceptual approach to teaching Chinese characters. Instructional Science, 35, 375–406.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, O. J. L. (2002). Current issues in learning to read Chinese. In M. Li, J. S. Gaffney, & J. L. Packard (Eds.), Chinese children’s reading acquisition: Theoretical and pedagogical issues (pp. 3–15). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. L. (1970). The structure of English orthography. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L. (1985). Zhōngguó xiàndài yŭfă [Modern Chinese grammar]. Beijing: Shangwu Yinshuguan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L. (1989). [Hanyu phonology] (2nd ed.) . Hong Kong: Zhonghua Shuju (in Chinese).

  • Wang, M., & Cheng, C. (2008). Subsyllabic unit preference in young Chinese children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 291–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L., & Tsou, H. L. (1999). Han Zi [Chinese characters]. Hong Kong: Hai Fung Publishers.

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2003). Alphabetic readers quickly acquire orthographic structure in learning to read Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 183–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, M., Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2004). The implicit and explicit learning of orthographic structure and function of a new writing system. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 357–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yin, W. G., & Weekes, B. S. (2003). Dyslexia in Chinese: Clues from cognitive neuropsychology. Annals of Dyslexia, 53, 255–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhōngguό guόjiā yǔwěi [Chinese National Language Committee] (1998). Xun xi chu li yong GB13000.1 zi fu ji: Hanzi bujian gui fan [Information processing using GB13000.1 Character symbol anthology: Hanzi bujian analyses]. Beijing: Beijing Language Institute Press.

Download references

Acknowledgment

The study was assisted in part with a grant from the Hong Kong Education Bureau (EDB). We thank HKEDB for its assistance. We also thank Hung Wai Ng, Man Ying Wong, the principals, teachers, and students of the cooperating schools. The views expressed in this paper are ours and do not necessarily represent those of HKEDB.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Che Kan Leong.

Appendix

Appendix

figure afigure a

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Leong, C.K., Loh, K.Y., Ki, W.W. et al. Enhancing orthographic knowledge helps spelling production in eight-year-old Chinese children at risk for dyslexia. Ann. of Dyslexia 61, 136–160 (2011). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-011-0051-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-011-0051-3

Keywords

Navigation