INTRODUCTION

Medical students are exposed to many situations during which they need to make important clinical decisions while feeling stressed or anxious.1 In such situations, their ability to take a medical history, perform a physical examination, and engage in clinical reasoning could be influenced by their emotional state.

According to Lazarus,2 stress (or distress) is experienced by an individual when the perceived demands of a situation exceed the perceived available resources to meet these demands. Conversely, a challenge feeling (eustress) appears when the perceived resources meet or exceed the perceived demands of the situation.

The effect of acute stress on clinical reasoning remains unclear. Most of the studies looking at the effects of stress have focused on the cognitive functions of attention, memory or decision-making in laboratory settings.35 The broader stress literature suggests an impact on memory retrieval and working memory according to the intensity of stress, with eustress being beneficial to performance compared to no stress or distress.6,7 In the relatively few studies conducted with health professionals, both enhancements and impairments have been reported, generally also as a function of the subjective and physiological responses to acute situations.3,8 Some data suggest that stress responses could induce premature closure in one’s initial hypothesis,9,10 which could lead to diagnostic errors.11

There is limited research suggesting that intrinsic stressors (integral—or tied—to the task being performed) could affect performance differently from extrinsic stressors (peripheral to the task being performed). When feeling stressed or anxious, a person’s cognitive resources will be biased towards the perceived source of threat.12 If the stressors are intrinsic to the task being performed, this results in the person’s attention and other cognitive resources being focused on the task. If, however, the stressors are extrinsic to the task, cognitive resources will be oriented towards those stressors at the expense of the task being performed. As such, integral stressors may facilitate performance by focusing attentional and other cognitive resources towards the task being performed.3 In the healthcare setting, extrinsic stressors (ES) can include noise and alarms,13 pain,14 disruptive participants (family member, colleague, team member),15 discord between participants,13,15 sleep deprivation,16 and social evaluation.17 In contrast, intrinsic stressors (IS) can include having to break bad news,18,19 high-stakes and urgent tasks,13,2022 dealing with uncertainty,23 and novel tasks.10 To date, there has been no research in health professional education comparing the effects of extrinsic and intrinsic stressors within the same cohort of learners.

The goal of this study was to compare the effects of extrinsic and intrinsic stressors on medical students’ clinical abilities (taking a history and performing a physical examination) and clinical reasoning. A secondary goal of this study was to examine the effects of perceived task difficulty independent of the effects of extrinsic and intrinsic stressors. We hypothesized that intrinsic stressors would enhance performance.

METHODS

Design of the Study (see Fig. 1)

Fig. 1
figure 1

Design of the study. Acute stress measures: cognitive appraisal, visual analogical scale, State-Trait Anxiety Inventory. Performance measures: ability to perform a clinical examination, communication skills, diagnostic accuracy, diagnostic argumentation. ES extrinsic stressor, IS intrinsic stressor

This was a prospective crossover study, with medical students conducting two medical ambulatory experiences with standardized patients (SPs). Volunteer third-year medical students were recruited through oral presentations made at the end of several large group lectures at the beginning of the academic year (September 2011) so that all the third-year students were aware of the study protocol. Written consent was provided and was collected from September to December 2011. The SP sessions were scheduled from February 20 to March 2, 2012. All students, regardless of their participation in the study, attended one SP session, which included four simulated experiences.

Frequent and previously taught medical problems were chosen (scenario A, dyspnea due to pulmonary embolism; scenario B, abdominal pain due to pancreatitis). Students had to conduct a clinical exam and generate diagnostic hypotheses based on the clinical data obtained during the clinical evaluation.

Students were not aware of the stress conditions they would encounter in their experiences. Before they were exposed to the two study experiences, the students performed two training consultations in order to become habituated to the task and to minimize the novelty effects of the situation (this was their first-ever SP session). Each experience lasted 15 min, with 5 min between each scenario.

The medical students’ subjective stress responses were assessed three times: 5 min before the first study experience (baseline), immediately after the first study experience (Time 2, or T2), and after the second study experience (Time 3, or T3).

Ethical Considerations

In the absence of ethics committees, for medical education research in France, the study protocol was read and approved by the dean of our faculty. The study protocol was also presented to a team of researchers in medical education at the Wilson Center in Toronto, Canada, for ethical review before the study was implemented. After receiving information regarding the details of the study protocol, students signed written consent forms. Data were analyzed anonymously. There was no incentive for participation. All students participated in the scenarios for the purpose of their education, but we collected data only from students who had consented to participate in the study.

Stressors

Extrinsic stressors were defined as stressful components peripheral to the task. Intrinsic stressors were defined as stressful components integral to the task itself. The task consisted of solving a clinical problem in a simulated ambulatory context.

Intrinsic Stressors (IS)

As previously demonstrated,13,20 “clinical severity” was chosen as an intrinsic stressor and included in scenario A (pulmonary embolism). Students were tasked with determining the correct diagnosis when faced with a patient presenting with acute dyspnea in the context of disseminated malignancy and palliative treatment.

Clinical severity was manifested such that it was immediately recognized by the student (high respiratory rate > 30 cycles/min and extreme difficulty talking). In no case was scenario A presented without signs of increased clinical severity. Moreover, the patient was aware of the diagnosis of cancer with metastatic dissemination and consequently expressed knowledge of the imminence of death. In addition to dyspnea, the patient was complaining of leg pain that he was certain was a new metastatic localization. He was also experiencing extreme fatigue. This particular condition was called “high clinical severity”. Conversely, scenario B had no such intrinsic stressor and was called “low clinical severity”. Scenario B consisted of a complaint of acute abdominal pain in a relatively healthy patient without any serious underlying pathology.

Extrinsic Stressors (ES)

Extrinsic stressors were incorporated into the scenarios in the following manner: The patient demonstrated lack of confidence in the student’s competency and demonstrated moderately aggressive behavior, such as reluctance to consult, impatience, and manifestations of negative mood. These extrinsic stressful conditions were applied to scenario A or B, according to the group assignment. This condition was called “aggressive patient”, and the opposite condition was called “pleasant patient”.

As such, the following conditions were created:

  1. 1)

    IS & ES: scenario A (high clinical severity) with an aggressive patient;

  2. 2)

    IS & no ES: scenario A (high clinical severity) with a pleasant patient;

  3. 3)

    ES & no IS: scenario B (low clinical severity) with an aggressive patient;

  4. 4)

    No ES & no IS: scenario B (low clinical severity) with a pleasant, cooperative patient.

In the first study scenario, students were randomly assigned to one of these four groups. In the second study scenario, students were allocated to the group having the opposite characteristics. Thus students were assigned to either a) condition 1 followed by condition 4, b) condition 2 followed by condition 3, c) condition 4 followed by condition 1, or d) condition 3 followed by condition 2.

Standardized Patient (SP) Training and Simulation of Ambulatory Setting

Eight professional actors were hired from a local theatre company. During a two-hour training session for each scenario, the actors were given a detailed description of the patient symptoms and character. The SPs were also trained regarding their demeanor for both the low-stress and the high-stress scenarios. Each item of the clinical skills checklist was carefully explained in order to describe the expected skill. The objective of the explanations was to teach the SPs to distinguish between different levels of performance and to accurately complete the Likert scales according to three levels (done, not done or partially done). For communication skills assessment, levels 0 and 100 of each analogical scale were described with behavioral anchors. All medical terms were described in detail with non-medical language, and any remaining questions were addressed to the satisfaction of the SPs.

The experiences were performed in an authentic environment, which included a desk, chairs, telephone, computer, and examination devices such as stethoscope, blood pressure cuff, and reflex hammer.

Assessment of Clinical Performance

Five components of clinical performance were assessed. Two of them were assessed by the SPs (see appendix):

  1. 1)

    Clinical abilities score (CLA): At the end of each experience, the SPs completed a checklist regarding the student’s performance in obtaining the clinical history and in performing the physical examination. The checklist contained 19 items for scenario A and 24 for scenario B. Each item was scored 0 (not done), 0.5 (partially done) or 1 (entirely done). The items of the checklist were established by two expert clinicians who were specialists in internal medicine, These two scores were transformed into a percentage of maximal total scores. In a previous study10 using the same assessment, the correlation coefficient between medical teachers and SPs and between two SPs of the same group of actors were 0.81 and 0.78, respectively.

  2. 2)

    Communication score (COS): The students’ communication skills were evaluated by the SPs on four analogical scales that assessed empathy, discourse coherence, and verbal and nonverbal communication.24 A global communication score was generated using the mean of the four items.

For this score, the correlation coefficients between medical teachers and SPs and between two SPs of the same group of actors were 0.58 and 0.6, respectively, in a previous study.10 Three components of clinical performance were assessed using written assessment forms completed by the student immediately following the experience and scored by one of the investigators based on the method described by LeBlanc25:

  1. 1)

    Diagnostic accuracy score (DA): Students were asked to indicate their clinical diagnosis for each experience. Diagnostic accuracy was scored on a three-point Likert scale (false or no diagnosis, partially correct, correct).

  2. 2)

    Positive arguments score (PAS): Students were asked to report the relevant clinical information supporting their main diagnosis. The maximum score was 5 for scenario A and 5 for scenario B.

  3. 3)

    Differential arguments score (DAS): Students were asked to report all relevant clinical information supporting differential diagnoses. The maximum was 4 for scenario A and 7 for scenario B.

The items used for scoring were selected by two expert clinicians who were specialists in internal medicine. These scores were transformed into a percentage of maximal total scores. The inter-rater correlations of these three latter scores were 0.92, 0.67 and 0.89, respectively, in a previous study.10

Subjective Stress Measures

Medical students’ subjective stress levels were assessed using three measures:

  • A cognitive appraisal (threat/challenge) score (CA)26 was assessed before and after each experience by calculating the ratio of primary appraisal (perceived demands) to secondary appraisal (perceived resources) for each student. Primary and secondary appraisal were respectively evaluated by the following questions, translated in French: “How demanding do you expect (or did you feel) the upcoming (or previous) task to be?” and “How are you able to (or did you) cope with this task?” Responses were recorded using a seven-point Likert-type scale. Threat appraisal was defined by a ratio of demands to resources greater than 1. In laboratory studies, threat appraisals have been associated with greater physiological stress responses than challenge appraisals.27

  • A visual analog scale (VAS) asking “quantify your stress from 0 (not stressed) to 100 (extremely stressed) on the following visual analog scale” was completed by the students before and after each experience. Such scales have been used as a marker of subjective stress in previous research.1,10

  • The French version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), developed by Bruchon-Schweitzer,28 measures anxiety experienced at a given moment. It includes 20 items scored from 1 to 4 on a Likert scale, with a total score ranging from 20 to 80. High internal consistency of the STAI (Cronbach’s α coefficient, 0.92) has been demonstrated.29

Potential Confounders

Previous research has shown that sex can influence both responses to stressors and the effects of stress responses on measures of cognitive ability.30 As such, we recorded each student’s sex, with men coded as 1 and women as 2. As the inclusion of the stressors could potentially affect the difficulty level of the experiences, the student-perceived cognitive difficulty of the scenarios was assessed 5 min after each experience using a visual analogical scale ranging from 0 (“very easy”) to 100 (“very difficult”). The question was the French equivalent of “From the clinical reasoning perspective, how difficult did you find this scenario?”

Sample Size

Given the absence of previous studies with similar designs and outcomes, no sample size calculation was performed. Thus the study was open to all third-year medical students at the University of Nantes (France).

Statistical Analysis

The three subjective stress measures (STAI, VAS, CA) and the five clinical performance measures described above were compared using multivariate mixed analyses of variance (ANOVA), with time (T2, T3) as the repeated measure and the following variables as fixed effects: baseline value (if available), presence of extrinsic and/or intrinsic stressors, sex, and student-perceived cognitive difficulty. Time and baseline value effects were imposed in the models even if these effects were not significant. In each model, the interactions between extrinsic and intrinsic stressors were tested. Significant effects were selected using a backward selection method. Results are presented in terms of adjusted mean differences on each studied variables (β parameters). For binary variables (time, sex, ES, IS) and for interactions ES x IS, they represent adjusted mean differences on each studied variable between the two groups of students (males and females, for example). For continuous variables (baseline, cognitive difficulty), they represent the gain or loss on each studied variable when the continuous variable increased by one unit. The signs of the β parameter correspond to the notion of increase (+) or decrease (−) of each studied variable between the groups coded 0 (females, no IS, no ES) and the groups coded 1 (males, presence of IS, presence of ES) or when a continuous variable increased by one unit. The significance level was fixed at 5 %. Statistical analyses were conducted using Stata 10 software.

RESULTS

One hundred nine students (49.5 % men, mean age 21.6 +/− 0.8 years) from a population of 236 third-year students participated in the study (response rate 46 %). There were no differences between participants and non-participants in terms of sex (p = 0.23), first-year ranking (p = 0.30), or number of registrations at university before the third year (p = 0.06). Participants were, on average, 4 months younger than non-participants (20.6 +/− 0.8 vs. 21.0 +/− 2.1, p = 0.04). There were no differences among the four groups of study participants in terms of mean age, sex, year 1 ranking, or pre-study measures of stress.

Means and confidence intervals according to the four stress conditions and time are displayed without any covariate adjustments in Table 1. Table 2 shows the independent effects of ES and IS, taking into account potential confounding variables.

Table 1 Means and confidence intervals of stress and clinical skills measures according to time and stressors
Table 2 Stress and performances measures—results of multivariate analysis (n =109)

Effects of Extrinsic Stressor (ES) and Intrinsic Stressor (IS)

Multivariate analysis showed that the presence of ES led to increased scores on the post-scenario STAI (β [regression parameter] = 3.9, p = 0.003) and higher scores on the two clinical skills measures: clinical abilities (β = 12.3, p < 0.001) and communication score (β = 15.4, p < 0.001). The presence of ES had no effect on the measures of clinical reasoning.

The presence of IS had no effect on either the stress or clinical skills measures. The presence of IS led to increases in diagnostic accuracy (β = 9.70, p = 0.004) and differential argumentation scores (β = 5.92, p = 0.02). The presence of IS conjointly with ES canceled the positive effect of ES, as reported above, for clinical abilities (p = 0.16) and communication score (p = 0.11) (data not shown).

Effect of Confounding Variables (Time, Sex, Order, Student-Perceived Cognitive Difficulty)

Students’ VAS and STAI scores, but not their CA scores, decreased over time (β = −7.3, p < 0.001 and −4.1, p < 0.001, respectively). Time had no effect on any of the performance measures. Communication skills scores were higher in female than in male students (β = −4.9, p = 0.04). An unexpected high level of student-perceived cognitive difficulty was found for both scenarios, but especially for scenario A (mean score of 46.5+/−21.0 for scenario B and 63.3 +/− 20.4 for scenario A), which had a substantial impact on stress and performance measures, with the exception of clinical abilities and communication scores (cf. Table 2). No effect was found for the order in which the students were exposed to intrinsic and extrinsic stressors.

DISCUSSION

The objective of this study was to evaluate the effects of intrinsic and extrinsic stressors on subjective stress responses and clinical performance of medical students. The results reveal that the student-perceived cognitive difficulty of the task emerged as a strong predictive factor for most of the stress and performance variables. When the effects of cognitive difficulty were accounted for in the analyses, the presence of extrinsic stressors increased subjective stress and improved clinical skills (clinical abilities and communication), while the presence of intrinsic stressors increased diagnostic accuracy and differential argumentation (i.e., the ability to evoke alternative diagnoses), without affecting stress responses. Furthermore, the presence of intrinsic stressors negated the beneficial effects of extrinsic stressors on clinical abilities when both were present. When presented together, ES and IS led to distress, while ES and IS alone led to eustress. It is also important to note that since we found no effect of the order of exposure to IS and ES, the effect caused by the presence of a stressor was of short duration. These results suggest that intrinsic and extrinsic stressors have different effects on subjective stress responses and on clinical skills and clinical reasoning abilities of medical students.

Some hypotheses can be put forward to explain the main findings (cf. Fig. 2) emerging from our multivariate analysis. The different effects of the intrinsic and extrinsic stressors can be explained based on the construct that stressors capture, or bias, cognitive resources.12 Intrinsic stressors, being intrinsically linked to a task, serve to focus cognitive resources towards the task (solving a clinical problem), which may explain the increase in diagnostic accuracy and differential argumentation. In contrast, the extrinsic stressors, the patient’s aggressive demeanor, likely oriented students’ cognitive resources towards the interpersonal aspects of the task, which in turn may result in increased communication and clinical abilities. It was as if a trainee, confronted with an aggressive patient, tried to demonstrate and make explicit his/her clinical abilities, which are competencies widely agreed as inherent to a good health professional.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Main findings and hypotheses

The theory of cognitive load or working memory limitations31 could explain that in our novice medical students, under IS, clinical reasoning was enhanced at the expense of clinical skills, and conversely, under ES, clinical skills were enhanced at the expense of clinical reasoning.

Our study is not the first to report an advantageous effect of stressors on clinical performance. In a reanimation setting, DeMaria15 found long-term improvement of clinical performance in students who had been exposed to extrinsic stressors 6 months before. LeBlanc also found improved performance under extrinsic stress conditions with emergency medicine22 and surgery residents.8 One possible explanation for these results is that under moderately stressful conditions, some cognitive functions such as memory and reasoning may be selectively enhanced, leading to an increase in some aspects of performance. Indeed, Buchanan6 found that the inclusion of stressors that did not induce a cortisol response led to enhanced memory retrieval, while cortisol responses were accompanied by impaired performance. Domes7 also showed an inverted-U type of performance curve as a function of cortisol response. In that study, mild responders showed higher performance than non- or high responders.

The results of the study should be interpreted within the context of several limitations. First, the effect of perceived cognitive difficulty was greater than expected. Although some of these effects could be accounted for statistically, it nevertheless could have disturbed the manipulations of intrinsic and extrinsic stressors and masked part of their effects. Second, although we selected common and previously taught clinical conditions for this study, the relatively high perceived cognitive difficulty suggests that the students may have lacked some knowledge for dealing with the scenarios, which may have influenced the observed effects of the studied stressors. Third, the generalizability of our findings is limited by the small number of scenarios and the fact that we chose to focus on a single variable potentially affecting clinical performance (i.e., acute stress). In real settings, clinical performance involves far more complex, interactive processes likely to alter the effects found in this study. Fourth, though the conceptual difference between ES and IS was clear, in our study the boundary between them was probably more tenuous. A questionnaire designed to ascertain whether students actually appraised IS and ES as expected might have been informative. Fifth, the student-perceived cognitive difficulty of the task was assessed with a single global rating immediately after the encounter, concurrently with stress measurement, which may have been insufficient to correctly assess this dimension and could have induced confusion with students’ overall stress.

In conclusion, this study showed that in the context of standardized patients, both extrinsic and intrinsic stressors had positive effects on clinical performance, but on different aspects. The results are consistent with the concept that stressors serve to capture cognitive resources and orient them towards sources of potential threat. Finally, our findings suggest that under stress conditions, medical students resort to an increased panel of clinical skills. Given that recent studies have demonstrated that stressful events are likely to enhance long-term memory and learning,3,15,32 an implication for clinical training is that regularly exposing students to ES and IS may be a useful educational tool for the long-term development of their clinical abilities and reasoning. Future research might look at the longer-term effects of exposing medical students to ES and IS. As was recommended in a recent review of the literature,33 such a study would inform the body of knowledge for a broader area of research investigating the effect of emotional state on cognitive processes.