Skip to main content
Log in

The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relationship between executive functions (inhibitory control and working memory) and metacognitive skills was investigated by applying correlational and regression analyses to data collected from two groups of children. To date, research in this area has lacked a theoretical model for considering these relationships; here we propose and test such a model. Our model hypothesises that if metacognitive skills are either monitoring or control processes, depending on the direction of information flow between the meta- and object-level, then each executive function should relate to one of these processes. Further, the contribution that executive functions and metacognitive skills make to educational achievement was examined. Results indicated that executive functions were more related to metacognitive skills in 5-year-olds than in 7-year-olds, and metacognitive skills were the most important predictors of educational achievement across both age groups. These data support an interpretation that the two skills are not identical to one another, and that executive functions could be ‘necessary but not sufficient’ antecedents to metacognitive skills, i.e. younger children’s immature executive functions may limit their metacognitive skill use.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note, in executive functioning literature the term ‘inhibition’ is also used.

  2. Sometimes also referred to as ‘updating’.

  3. Please note: Within each age group, there were no significant differences between the genders in any of the outcome scores.

References

  • Alloway, T. P., & Archibald, L. M. D. (2008). Working memory and learning in children with developmental coordination disorder and specific language impairment. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(3), 251–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alloway, T. P., Gathercole, S. E., & Pickering, S. J. (2007). Automated working memory assessment. Durham: University of Durham.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., Samson, D., & Humphreys, G. W. (2009). Studies of adults can inform accounts of theory of mind development. Developmental Psychology, 45(1), 190–201. doi:10.1037/a0014098.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A., & Hitch, G. J. (1974). Working memory. In G. A. Bower (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation. New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bekci, B., & Karakas, S. (2006). Metacognition and executive functions: Related or different concepts. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 61(3), 343–343.

    Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, C., & Razza, R. P. (2007). Relating effortful control, executive function, and false belief understanding to emerging math and literacy ability in kindergarten. Child Development, 78(2), 647–663. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01019.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blais, C., Robidoux, S., Risko, E. F., & Besner, D. (2007). Item specific adaptation and the conflict monitoring hypothesis: A computational model. Psychological Review, 114, 1076–1086.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Botvinick, M. M., Cohen, J. D., & Carter, C. S. (2004). Conflict monitoring and anterior cingulate cortex: An update. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 539–546.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryce, D., & Whitebread, D. (2012). The development of metacognitive skill: Evidence from observational analysis of young children’s behavior during problem-solving. Metacognition and Learning, 7(3), 197–217. doi:10.1007/s11409-012-9091-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryce, D., Szucs, D., Soltesz, F., & Whitebread, D. (2011). The development of inhibitory control: An averaged and single-trial lateralized readiness potential study. NeuroImage, 57, 671–685. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.12.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bunge, S. A., Dudukovic, N. M., Thomason, M. E., Vaidya, C. J., & Gabriela, J. D. E. (2002). Immature frontal lobe contributions to cognitive control in children: Evidence from fMRI. Neuron, 33, 301–311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jager, B., Jansen, M., & Reezigt, G. (2005). The development of metacognition in primary school learning environments. School Effectiveness and School Improvement, 16(2), 179–196. doi:10.1080/09243450500114181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deak, G. O., & Narasimham, G. (2003). Is perseveration caused by inhibition failure? Evidence from preschool children’s inferences about word meanings. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 194–222. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2003.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, A., Barnett, W. S., Thomas, J., & Munro, S. (2007). The early years—Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science, 318(5855), 1387–1388. doi:10.1126/science.1151148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dignath, C., & Buettner, G. (2008). Components of fostering self-regulated learning among students. A meta-analysis on intervention studies at primary and secondary school level. Metacognition and Learning, 3(23), 1–264.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dignath, C., Buettner, G., & Langfeldt, H. (2008). How can primary school students learn self-regulated learning strategies most effectively? A meta-analysis on self-regulation training programmes. Educational Research Review, 3, 101–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, J., Emslie, H., Williams, P., Johnson, R., & Freer, C. (1996). Intelligence and the frontal lobe: The organization of goal-directed behavior. Cognitive Psychology, 30(3), 257–303. doi:10.1006/cogp.1996.0008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Efklides, A. (2008). Metacognition: Defining its facets and levels of functioning in relation to self-regulation and co-regulation. European Psychologist, 13(4), 277–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Executive attention and metacognitive regulation. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 288–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher, L., & Carruthers, P. (2012). Metacognition and reasoning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, B: Biological Sciences, 367, 1366–1378.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garner, J. K. (2009). Conceptualizing the relations between executive functions and self-regulated learning. The Journal of Psychology, 143(4), 405–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J., Dunlosky, J., & Graesser, A. C. (1998). Metacognition in educational theory and practice. Mahwah, N. J.: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hofmann, W., Schmeichel, B. J., & Baddeley, A. (2012). Executive functions and self-regulation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(3), 174–180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huizinga, M., Dolan, C. V., & van der Molen, M. W. (2006). Age-related change in executive function: Developmental trends and a latent variable analysis. Neuropsychologia, 44(11), 2017–2036.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jongen, E. M. M., & Jonkman, L. M. (2008). The developmental pattern of stimulus and response interference in a color-object Stroop task: an ERP study. BMC Neuroscience, 9(82), 1–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kistner, S., Rakoczy, K., Otto, B., Dignath-van Ewijk, C., Buttner, G., & Klieme, E. (2010). Promotion of self-regulated learning in classrooms: investigating frequency, quality, and consequences for student performance. Metacognition and Learning, 5(2), 157–171. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9055-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D. (2000). Metacognitive development. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9(5), 178–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Pease, M. (2010). The dual components of developing strategy use. In H. Salatas Waters & W. Schneider (Eds.), Metacognition, strategy use and instruction. New York: The Guildford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lansbergen, M., van Hell, E., & Kenemans, J. L. (2007). Impulsivity and conflict in the Stroop task. Journal of Psychophysiology, 21(2), 33–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84(6), 1933–1953. doi:10.1111/Cdev.12096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzer, L. (2007). Executive function in education: From theory to practice. London: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21(1), 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41(1), 49–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J. B., & Munakata, Y. (2002). Active versus latent representations: A neural network model of perseveration, dissociation, and decalage. Developmental Psychobiology, 40(3), 255–265. doi:10.1002/Dev.10033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. Bower (Ed.), The Psychology of Learning and Motivation (Vol. 26). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuenschwander, R., Cimeli, P., Rothlisberger, M., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Personality factors in elementary school children: Contributions to academic performance over and above executive functions? Learning and Individual Differences, 25, 118–125. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennequin, V., Sorel, O., & Mainguy, M. (2010). Metacognition, executive functions and aging: The effect of training in the use of metacognitive skills to solve mathematical word problems. Journal of Adult Development, 17(3), 168–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrotin, A., Belleville, S., & Isingrini, M. (2007). Metamemory monitoring in mild cognitive impairment: Evidence of a less accurate episodic feeling-of-knowing. Neuropsychologia, 45, 2811–2826.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perrotin, A., Tournelle, L., & Isingrini, M. (2008). Executive functioning and memory as potential mediators of the episodic feeling-of-knowing accuracy. Brain and Cognition, 67, 76–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, N. E. (1998). Young children’s self-regulated learning and contexts that support it. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1998(90), 4.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pino Pasternak, D., Whitebread, D., & Tolmie, A. (2010). A multidimensional analysis of parent–child interactions during academic tasks and their relationships with children. Cognition and Instruction, 28(3), 219–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning P. R. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation doi:http://lib.myilibrary.com?ID=92724.

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Kelley, C. M. (2005). Executive processes, memory accuracy, and memory monitoring: An aging and individual difference analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 578–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Cimeli, P., Roethlisberger, M., & Neuenschwander, R. (2012). Executive functioning, metacognition, and self-perceived competence in elementary school children: An explorative study on their interrelations and their role for school achievement. Metacognition and Learning, 7, 151–173. doi:10.1007/s11409-012-9089-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Vise, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000). Developmental trends in children’s memory monitoring—Evidence from a judgment-of-learning task. Cognitive Development, 15(2), 115–134. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00024-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schunk, D. H. (2001). Social cognitive theory and self-regulated learning. In B. J. Zimmerman & D. H. Schunk (Eds.), Self-regulated learning and academic achievement: Theoretical perspectives (pp. 125–151). NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Souchay, C., & Isingrini, M. (2004). Age related differences in metacognitive control: Role of executive functioning. Brain and Cognition, 56, 89–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Souchay, C., Isingrini, M., & Espagnet, L. (2000). Aging, episodic memory feeling-of-knowing, and frontal functioning. Neuropsychology, 14(2), 299–309. doi:10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szucs, D., Soltesz, F., Bryce, D., & Whitebread, D. (2009). Real-time tracking of motor response activation and response competition in a Stroop task in young children: A lateralized readiness potential study. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(11), 2195–2206. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taconnat, L., Raz, N., Tocze, C., Bouazzaoui, B., Sauzeon, H., Fay, S., & Isingrini, M. (2009). Ageing and organisation strategies in free recall: The role of cognitive flexibility. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 21(2–3), 347–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2010). Development of metacognitive skillfulness: A longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 20(3), 220–224. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2009.11.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Wilhelm, P., & Beishuizen, J. J. (2004). The relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills from a developmental perspective. Learning and Instruction, 14(1), 89–109. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2003.10.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., Van Hout-Wolters, B. H. A. M., & Afflerbach, P. (2006). Metacognition and learning: conceptual and methodological considerations. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 3–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vukman, K. B., & Licardo, M. (2010). How cognitive, metacognitive, motivational and emotional self-regulation influence school performance in adolescence and early adulthood. Educational Studies, 36(3), 259–268. doi:10.1080/03055690903180376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2005). Wechsler Individual Achievement Test (WIAT II) (2nd ed.). London: The Psychological Corp.

  • Whitebread, D. (2013). Self-regulation in young children: Its characteristics and the role of communication and language in its early development. In D. Whitebread, N. Mercer, C. Howe, & A. Tolmie (Eds.), Self-regulation and dialogue in primary classrooms. British journal of educational psychology monograph series II: Psychological aspects of education – current trends, No. 10 (pp. 25–44). Leicester: BPS.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Pino Pasternak, D., Sangster, C., Grau, V., Bingham, S., & Demetriou, D. (2009). The development of two observational tools for assessing metacognition and self-regulated learning in young children. Metacognition and Learning, 4(1), 63–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Whitebread, D., Pino Pasternak, D., Bryce, D., Vuillier, L., Buettner, G., Perels, F., & Shamir, A. (2011). Identifying and measuring self-regulation in young children: The development and validation of the CHILD observational instrument. UK: Paper presented at the EARLI Biennial Conference University of Exeter.

    Google Scholar 

  • Willoughby, M. T., Blair, C. B., Wirth, R. J., Greenberg, M., & Family Life Project Investigators. (2012). The measurement of executive function at age 5: Psychometric properties and relationship to academic achievement. Psychological Assessment, 24(1), 226–239. doi:10.1037/A0025361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Winne, P. H., & Hadwin, A. F. (1998). Studying as self-regulated learning. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 279–306). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Muller, U., Frye, D., & Marcovitch, S. (2003). The development of executive function. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 68(3), 1–137. doi:10.1111/j.0037-976X.2003.00261.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive perspective P. R. Pintrich, M. Boekaerts & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation doi: http://lib.myilibrary.com?ID=92724.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This work was funded by an ESRC PhD studentship grant awarded to D.B.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Donna Bryce.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bryce, D., Whitebread, D. & Szűcs, D. The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children. Metacognition Learning 10, 181–198 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4

Keywords

Navigation