Skip to main content
Log in

On the “Exporting” of Morality: Its Relation to Political Conservatism and Epistemic Motivation

  • Published:
Social Justice Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A new phenomenon, moral exporting (ME), is introduced to capture active attempts to promote certain views of morality to others. It was hypothesized that political conservatives would be more likely to exhibit ME, due in part to strong epistemic concerns for certainty that may become attached to the moral domain. Related items from the 1988 and 2006 General Social Surveys were analyzed, and new scales were developed to better assess ME and specific moral-related epistemic concerns (moral absolutism). In a second study, these scales were administered to a large college student sample along with measures of political ideology and need for closure (NFC). Results generally showed that political conservatism was strongly related to the new ME factor. Further analysis determined that both moral absolutism (MA) and NFC accounted for significant portions of this relationship, but that the specific epistemic construct (MA) was a more proximal mediator. Discussion centers on further distinguishing ME and MA from related constructs, as well as on future research and applications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It is important to note that this is only one of several strategies an individual may employ to deal with the threat of uncertainty that moral diversity inherently stimulates. Some may choose to avoid, or even actively derogate, those with different views. This will be discussed further in the General Discussion.

  2. Once again, some moral absolutists may choose to deal with such threats by avoiding and/or derogating those individuals and groups who elicit such uncertainty. However, others may choose the more benevolent “exporting” strategy.

  3. In order to examine if the basic pattern of results differed across the 1988 and 2006 data sets, we also examined if survey year interacted with political orientation and MA in predicting ME. It did not (both Wald χ2s < .15, ns).

  4. These open-ended responses were not analyzed for this article, as the focus here is on the construct of ME independent of content. It may be important to consider differences in content as related to political ideology in future research, and this is considered further in the General Discussion.

  5. In addition to ME and MA, Mormons also scored the highest on the other constructs in our model: conservatism (Mormons: M = 4.32, SD = 1.18; other religious: M = 3.26, SD = 1.31; t(393) = 8.28, p < .01) and NFC (Mormons: M = 3.79, SD = .39; other religious: M = 3.69, SD = .46; t(393) = 2.16, p < .05). Overall, religious participants were more conservative than non-religious participants, though there were no differences in NFC.

  6. As a covariate, NFC was a significant predictor of ME (b = .37, SE = .09, PRE = .03) and MA (b = .63, SE = .10, PRE = .06), controlling for age, sex, and monthly religious attendance. When testing a mediation model similar to that reported in the main results, NFC was a weak but significant mediator of the conservatism–ME relationship. In this model, conservatism significantly predicted NFC (b = .09, SE = .01, p < .01, PRE = .08), and NFC predicted greater ME over and above the effects of conservatism (b = .20, SE = .09, p < .05, PRE = .01). Although 18.5% of the direct effect of conservatism on ME was mediated in this model (Sobel z = 2.06, p < .05), more conservative identification continued to strongly predict ME, b = .19, SE = .03, p < .01, PRE = .08. The fact that MA strongly predicted ME even when controlling for NFC (with NFC becoming a nonsignificant predictor, b = .07, SE = .08, p > .05) suggests that MA is the more proximal construct here. It appears that while general epistemic concerns are associated with adopting an ME strategy in relation to others, this is fully accounted for by the more specific epistemic concerns within the moral domain (MA).

References

  • Altemeyer, B. (1998). The other “Authoritarian Personality”. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 30, pp. 47–91). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Altemeyer, B., & Hunsberger, B. (1992). Authoritarianism, religious fundamentalism, quest, and prejudice. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 2, 113–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Asch, S. E. (1955). Opinions and social pressure. Scientific American, 193, 31–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J. (2003). Value analysis of political behavior—Self-interested: Moralistic: Altruistic: Moral. University of Pennsylvania Law Review, 151, 1135–1167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baumeister, R. F. (1998). The self. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), The handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 680–740). New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bennett, J., & Shapiro, M. J. (2002). The politics of moralizing. New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chirumbolo, A., Livi, S., Mannetti, L., Pierro, A., & Kruglanski, A. W. (2004). Effects of need for closure on creativity in small group interactions. European Journal of Personality, 18, 265–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Choi, J., & Bowles, S. (2007). The coevolution of parochial altruism and war. Science, 318, 636–640.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Conover, P., & Feldman, S. (1981). The origins and meaning of liberal/conservative self-identifications. American Journal of Political Science, 25, 617–645.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L. (1954). A theory of social comparison processes. Human Relations, 7, 117–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Festinger, L., Riecken, H. W., & Schachter, S. (1956). When prophecy fails. Minneapolis, MN: University of Minnesota Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Frenkel-Brunswick, E. (1948). Intolerance of ambiguity as an emotional and perceptual personality variable. Journal of Personality, 18, 108–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glasman, L. R., & Albarracín, D. (2006). Forming attitudes that predict future behavior: A meta-analysis of the attitude–behavior relation. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 778–822.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Greenberg, J., Pyszczynski, T., Solomon, S., Rosenblatt, A., Veeder, M., Kirkland, S., et al. (1990). Evidence for terror management II: The effects of mortality salience on reactions to those who threaten or bolster the cultural worldviews. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58, 308–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greenwald, A. G. (1980). The totalitarian ego: Fabrication and revision of personal history. American Psychologist, 35, 603–618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grieve, P. G., & Hogg, M. A. (1999). Subjective uncertainty and intergroup discrimination in the minimal group situation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25, 926–940.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guth, J. L., & Green, J. C. (1991). An ideology of rights: Support for civil liberties among political activists. Political Behavior, 13, 321–344.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20, 98–116.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2009). Planet of the Durkheimians, where community, authority, and sacredness are foundations of morality. In J. Jost, A. C. Kay, & H. Thorisdottir (Eds.), Social and psychological bases of ideology and system justification. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, C. D., & Conley, T. D. (2001). A relational approach to cognition: Shared experience and relationship affirmation in social cognition. In G. Moskowitz (Ed.), Cognitive social psychology: Princeton Symposium on the legacy and future of social cognition (pp. 3–17). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardin, C. D., & Higgins, E. T. (1996). Shared reality: How social verification makes the subjective objective. In R. M. Sorrentino & E. T. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition (Vol. 3, pp. 28–84). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hollander, B. (1997). Fuel to the fire: Talk radio and the Gamson Hypothesis. Political Communication, 14, 355–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Janoff-Bulman, R., & Sheikh, S. (2006). From national trauma to moralizing nation. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 325–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T. (2006). The end of the end of ideology. American Psychologist, 61, 1–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129, 339–375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1981). Process analysis: Estimating mediation in treatment evaluations. Evaluation Review, 5, 602–619.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, A. (2002). Generating a virtuous circle: Democratic identity, moralism, and the languages of political responsibility. In J. Bennett & M. J. Shapiro (Eds.), The politics of moralizing (pp. 27–61). New York: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kellstedt, L. A., Green, J. C., Guth, J. L., & Smidt, C. E. (1996). Religious voting blocs in the 1992 election: The year of the evangelical? In J. C. Green (Ed.), Religion and the culture wars: Dispatches from the front (pp. 267–290). Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1989). Lay epistemics and human knowledge: Cognitive and motivational biases. New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W. (1990). Motivations for judging and knowing: Implications for causal attribution. In E. T. Higgins & R. M. Sorrentino (Eds.), The handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundation of social behavior (Vol. 2, pp. 333–368). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Pierro, A., Mannetti, L., & DeGrada, E. (2006). Groups as epistemic providers: Need for closure and the unfolding of group-centrism. Psychological Review, 113, 84–100.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Shah, J. Y., Pierro, A., & Mannetti, L. (2002). When similarity breeds content: Need for closure and the allure of homogenous and self-resembling groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 648–662.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kruglanski, A. W., Webster, D. M., & Klem, A. (1993). Motivated resistance and openness to persuasion in the presence or absence of prior information. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65, 861–876.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lun, J., Sinclair, S., Whitchurch, E. R., & Glenn, C. (2007). Why do I think what you think? Epistemic social tuning and implicit prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 957–972.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, I. (2006). Zeal appeal: The allure of moral extremes. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28, 343–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, I., Nail, P. R., Marigold, D. C., & Kang, S. (2005). Defensive pride and consensus: Strength in imaginary numbers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 978–996.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McGregor, I., Zanna, M. P., Holmes, J. G., & Spencer, S. J. (2001). Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: Going to extremes and being oneself. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80, 472–488.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, W. N., Worchel, S., Bois, J. L., Pearson, J. A., Rountree, C. A., Samaha, G. M., et al. (1976). Collective coping with stress: Group reactions to fear, anxiety, and ambiguity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 33, 674–679.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pergament, K. I. (2002). The bitter and the sweet: An evaluation of the costs and benefits of religiousness. Psychological Inquiry, 13, 168–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peterson, B. (2008). Goals and groups: Testing a functional (defensive) self-regulatory model of group identification and intergroup attitudes. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Utah.

  • Reid, S. A., & Hogg, M. A. (2005). Uncertainty reduction, self-enhancement, and ingroup identification. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 804–817.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodewalt, F., & Peterson, B. (2008). The self and social behavior: The fragile self and interpersonal self-regulation. In F. Rhodewalt (Ed.), Personality and social behavior. New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rozin, P. (1999). The process of moralization. Psychological Science, 10, 218–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schachter, S., & Singer, J. (1962). Cognitive, social, and physiological determinants of emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 379–399.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, J. Y., Kruglanski, A. W., & Thompson, E. P. (1998). Membership has its (epistemic) rewards: Need for closure effects on in-group bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 383–393.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sherif, M. (1936). The psychology of social norms. Oxford: Harper.

  • Silberman, I. (2005). Religion as a meaning system: Implications for the new millennium. Journal of Social Issues, 61, 641–663.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Skitka, L. J., Bauman, C. W., & Sargis, E. G. (2005). Moral conviction: Another contributor to attitude strength or something more? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88, 895–917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, C. (2003). Moral, believing animals: Human personhood and culture. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1986). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In S. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of Intergroup Relations (pp. 7–24). Chicago, IL: Nelson-Hall Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tavris, C., & Aronson, E. (2007). Mistakes were made (but not by me): Why we justify foolish beliefs, bad decisions, and hurtful acts. Orlando, FL: Harcourt, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tetlock, P. E. (1994). Political psychology or politicized psychology: Is the road to scientific hell paved with good moral intentions? Political Psychology, 15, 509–529.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomkins, S. S. (1963). Left and right. A basic dimension of ideology and personality. In R. W. White (Ed.), The study of lives (pp. 388–411). Chicago: Atherton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C., Hogg, M. A., Oakes, P. J., Reicher, S. D., & Wetherall, M. S. (1987). Rediscovering the social group: A self-categorization theory. Oxford and New York: Basil Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turner, J. C., Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & McGarty, C. A. (1994). Self and collective: Cognition and social context. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20, 454–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K., Euwema, M. C., Poortvliet, P. M., & Maas, M. (2007). Uncertainty management and social issues: Uncertainty as an important determinant of reactions to socially deviating people. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 37, 1726–1756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Vugt, M., De Cremer, D., & Janssen, D. P. (2007). Gender differences in cooperation and competition: The male-warrior hypothesis. Psychological Science, 18, 19–23.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Webster, D. M., & Kruglanski, A. W. (1994). Individual differences in need for cognitive closure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 1049–1062.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G. D., & Patterson, J. R. (1968). A new measure of conservatism. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 7, 264–269.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to express their gratitude to the Society for Personality and Social Psychology for organizing our 2007 Summer Institute in Social Psychology in Austin, TX (with funding from the National Science Foundation), and to both John Jost and Arie Kruglanski, who were the facilitators of our workshop on Political Ideology. The ideas for this research were formulated within this workshop during the two-week institute. We also thank Fred Rhodewalt, Art Brief, and the CU Stereotyping and Prejudice lab for comments on an earlier version of this manuscript, as well as Chick Judd for statistical consultation. Finally, we thank all anonymous reviewers for their thoughtful comments. It should be noted that the first two authors of this manuscript are listed in alphabetical order, reflecting their shared contributions.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Benjamin Peterson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Peterson, B., Smith, J.A., Tannenbaum, D. et al. On the “Exporting” of Morality: Its Relation to Political Conservatism and Epistemic Motivation. Soc Just Res 22, 206–230 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-009-0101-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-009-0101-8

Keywords

Navigation